ORIGINALARTICLE
IntimatePartnerViolenceandSubsequentDepression:
ExaminingtheRolesofNeighborhoodSupportiveMechanisms
EmilyM.Wright
1 • GillianM.Pinchevsky2 • MichaelL.Benson3 •
DanaL.Radatz4
Publishedonline:21September2015
SocietyforCommunityResearchandAction2015
Abstract Thisstudyexaminesthedirecteffectsof
neighborhoodsupportivemechanisms(e.g.,collective
efficacy,socialcohesion,socialnetworks)ondepressive
symptomsamongfemalesaswellastheirmoderating
effectsontheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepressive
symptoms.Amultilevel,multivariateRaschmodelwas
usedwithdatafromtheProjectonHumanDevelopmentin
ChicagoNeighborhoodstoassesstheexistenceofIPVand
latersusceptibilityofdepressivesymptomsamong2959
adultfemalesin80neighborhoods.Resultsindicatethat
neighborhoodcollectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,social
interactions,andthenumberoffriendsandfamilyinthe
neighborhoodreducethelikelihoodthatfemalesexperi-
encedepressivesymptoms.However,livinginareaswith
highproportionsoffriendsandrelativesexacerbatesthe
impactofIPVonfemales’subsequentdepressivesymp-
toms.Thefindingsindicatethatneighborhoodsupportive
mechanismsimpactinterpersonaloutcomesinbothdirect
andmoderatingways,althoughdirecteffectsweremore
pronouncedfordepressionthanmoderatingeffects.Future
researchshouldcontinuetoexaminethepositiveand
potentiallymitigatinginfluencesofneighborhoodsinorder
tobetterunderstandforwhomandunderwhichcircum-
stancesviolentrelationshipsandmentalhealthareinflu-
encedbycontextualfactors.
Keywords Intimatepartnerviolence Depression
Neighborhoods Protectivefactors Collectiveefficacy
Socialties
Introduction
Intimatepartnerviolence(IPV)anddepressionaremajor
publichealthconcernsthatarebothimpactedbyneigh-
borhoodcontext(Bensonetal. 2003;Kim 2008;Mairetal.
2008;Miles-Doan 1998;PinchevskyandWright 2012;
Ross 2000).Themajorityofextantresearch,however,has
tendedtofocusonthedirectandnegativeimpactof
neighborhoodsontheseoutcomes,whilelittleresearchhas
exploredthepotentialprotectiveeffectsof positive neigh-
borhoodconditions.Forinstance,thereissomeresearch
whichsuggestssupportivemechanismssuchassocial
cohesion,socialties,orcollectiveefficacymayprotect
individualsfromIPVordepressionandothermentalhealth
issues(AneshenselandSucoff 1996;Emeryetal. 2011;
GeisandRoss 1998;Kirstetal. 2015;Pinchevskyand
Wright 2012;RossandJang 2000;Rossetal. 2000;Wright
andBenson 2010, 2011),butmostofthesestudiesassess
onlyneighborhooddirecteffectsandtreattheseoutcomes
& EmilyM.Wright
[email protected]
GillianM.Pinchevsky
[email protected]
MichaelL.Benson
[email protected]
DanaL.Radatz
[email protected]
1
SchoolofCriminologyandCriminalJustice,University
ofNebraska,6001DodgeStreet,218CPACS,Omaha,
NE68182-0149,USA
2
DepartmentofCriminalJustice,UniversityofNevada,
LasVegas,4505SouthMarylandParkway,
Box455009,LasVegas,NV89154,USA
3
SchoolofCriminalJustice,UniversityofCincinnati,
665HDyerHall,Cincinnati,OH45221,USA
4
DepartmentofCriminology&CriminalJustice,Niagara
University,P.O.Box1941,NewYork,NY14109-1941,USA
123
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
DOI10.1007/s10464-015-9753-8separately.Mostnotablyforthecurrentstudy,verylittle
researchhasinvestigatedthepotentialforneighborhood
supportivefeaturestomoderateimportantindividual-level
relationships,suchastheimpactofIPVonsubsequent
depression.
Thus,whileweknowthatneighborhoodscandetri-
mentallyimpactIPVanddepression(LatkinandCurry
2003;Mairetal. 2008;PinchevskyandWright 2012),we
knowlessaboutthepositiveimpactthatneighborhood
supportivefeaturesmighthaveontheseproblems.Addi-
tionally,wedonotknowwhether,orhow,neighborhood
supportivemechanismsmoderatetheimpactofIPVon
subsequentdepression.Weattempttoaddtothesparse
researchinthisareabyexaminingtherelationshipbetween
intimatepartnerviolenceandsubsequentdepressionin
neighborhoodcontext.Weusedatafrom2959females
livingin80neighborhoodsinChicagotoinvestigatethe
directeffectsofneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,
suchascollectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,andinterac-
tionswithothers,onfemales’depressivesymptoms,as
wellastoexplorewhetherneighborhoodcontextalleviates
thelong-termimpactofexperiencingIPVonlater
depressionamongtheseadults.
IntimatePartnerViolenceVictimization
andDepression
Scholarshavesuggestedthatvictimsofpartnerviolence
maybeparticularlysusceptibletoexperiencingmental
healthproblemsintheaftermathofabuseinpartbecause
theirvictimization(andtheassociatedtrauma)isinflicted
bypeoplewhomthevictimtrusts,loves,orconsiderstobe
‘‘safe’’(DeMarisandKaukinen 2005).Alongwithpost-
traumaticstressdisorder,depressionisamongthemost
prevalentmentalhealthconsequencesofIPV(Caldwell
etal. 2012;Campbell 2002),withover45%ofvictims
experiencingit(Golding 1999).Althoughdepression
amongvictimsmaydeclineifandwhenthevictimization
does,long-termeffectsdolinger,butmoreresearchinthis
areaisneeded,especiallyamongadultpopulations
(Bonomietal. 2006;Campbell 2002;Cokeretal. 2002a;
Fletcher 2010;Johnsonetal. 2014).
Violencefrompartnerscanbelinkedtodepressionvia
mechanismssuchasstress,powerlessness,hopelessness,
isolation,lowself-esteem,andphysicalpain(Campbell
etal. 1996;Goodmanetal. 2009).Asasourceofstress,
abusecancontributetodepressionbydisruptingdaily
routines,increasingotherstressfuleventsinone’slife,
loweringthevictim’sfeelingsofsecurityandsenseofself-
esteem,orincreasingtheirfeelingsofpowerlessnessto
controlthesituation(Campbell 2002;Campbelletal. 1996;
Goodmanetal. 2009).Forinstance,becauseofthephysical
painandinjurythatisoftenaconsequenceofIPV,victims
maydevelopchronicpainorsufferconsequencesofthe
abuse(e.g.,episodesoffaintingcausedbyextensivehead
trauma;sexuallytransmitteddiseases,etc.,seeCampbell
2002),whichmayleadtodepressionovertime.Inaddition,
socialisolationfromothers,whichisoftenpurposefully
achievedbyabusersinviolentrelationships(e.g.,Campbell
etal. 1996),canalsoleadtodepressionbecauseisolated
victimsmayfeelthattheyhavenoonetoturntoforhelpor
support.WhiletherearemanyavenuesbywhichIPVmay
leadtodepressionandothernegativementalhealthdis-
positions,fewstudieshaveexaminedsuchrelationships
withlongitudinaldataamongadults,andmoreassessment
isneededinthisarea(Campbell 2002;Fletcher 2010).
Moreover,researchhasneglectedtoexaminethisrela-
tionshipfromacontextualperspective.
TheImportanceofNeighborhoodContext
Neighborhoodcontextisrelatedtodepressionandother
problematicmentalhealthoutcomesamongbothadults
(e.g.,Kim 2008;Mairetal. 2008)andchildren(e.g.,Xue
etal. 2005).Theliteratureregardingtheneighborhood
predictorsofdepressiontendstocenteronsocioeconomic,
poverty-related,orsocialdisordervariables,andgenerally
demonstratesthatlowersocioeconomicstatus(SES)and
moreneighborhooddisorderdetrimentallyimpactdepres-
sionandothernegativementalhealthoutcomes.Mairetal.
(2008)foundthat,of45studiesonneighborhoodsand
depression,37demonstratedsignificantneighborhood
effects,evenaftercontrollingforindividual-levelfactors
suchasage,race,gender,andindicatorsofsocioeconomic
status;inanotherreview,Kim(2008)foundthat11of22
studiesexaminingneighborhoodsocioeconomicstatus
showedthatneighborhoodconditionshadsignificant
effectsondepression.Mechanismsdirectlylinkingneigh-
borhoodfactorstomentalhealthordepressionoften
revolvearoundincreasedstresslevels,limitedaccesstoor
scantresources,disorder,violence,inadequatehousing,
andalackofpublicaccessorgreenspaces,suchasbike
lanesandcleanstreetsorsidewalks(e.g.,Kim 2008;Mair
etal. 2008;TaylorandRepetti 1997).Accumulatingevi-
dencealsosuggeststhatperceiveddisorderand/ordan-
gerousnessoftheneighborhoodincreasesdepression,
perhapsinpartduetoincreasedfearormistrustamong
neighbors(e.g.,RossandJang 2000;RossandMirowsky
2009),socialisolation(e.g.,GeisandRoss 1998;Rossand
Mirowsky 2009),feelingsofpowerlessness(e.g.,Anesh-
enselandSucoff 1996;GeisandRoss 1998),orfearand
anxietyamongstneighbors(e.g.,Hilletal. 2005).Inshort,
unkempt,rundown,disorderly,anddisorganizedneigh-
borhoodenvironmentscanbedemoralizing,distressing,
threatening,andisolatingtoresidentsandcansendthe
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 343
123messagethatnoonecaresabouttheirwellbeingorthatof
theneighborhood(RossandMirowsky 2009;Sampson
2013;WilsonandKelling 1982).Allofthesefactorsmight,
inturn,contributetodepression.
Despitethedeleteriouseffectsofneighborhoodson
negativeoutcomes,itisalsopossiblethatneighborhoods
may mitigate theeffectsofstressors—includingviolence
suchasIPV—byorganizingcommunityresidentstogether
andfosteringsocialcohesionand/orinterconnectedness
amongthem.Thereissomeevidencesupportingneigh-
borhoodprotectiveeffectsagainstmentalhealthoutcomes,
butthisbodyofresearchprimarilyfocusesonneighbor-
hoodcohesionandcollectiveefficacy.Intheirreview,Mair
etal.(2008)found11studiesthatreportedthatpositive
interactionsbetweenneighbors—includingincreasedsocial
connectionsandsupportfromresidents,increasedcom-
municationbetweenresidents,andreducedsocialisola-
tion—servedasprotectivefactorsagainstdepression.Ross
andcolleagues(KimandRoss 2009;RossandJang 2000)
havedemonstratedthatneighborhoodsocialtiesandsocial
supportreducedepressionandotherpsychologicaldistress,
includingperceivedpowerlessness,fear,andmistrust(see
alsoKim 2010).Neighborhoodsocialcohesionhasalso
beenlinkedtoreduceddepression(Mairetal. 2010);Ahern
andGalea(2011)suggestthatsocialcohesionwithina
neighborhoodmayhelpresidentsexertsocialcontrol,
whichcanprovidemoresupportbetweenneighbors,reduce
thenumberofstressorsthatresidentsperceivetoexist
withintheneighborhood,andbufferormitigatethestres-
sorswhichdooccur.Further,theynotethatcohesion
betweenresidentsmayfostercommunication—whichcan
keepresidentsknowledgeableabouttheircommunity—
improvethelocalservicesandresourcesthatareavailable,
andincreaseapersonalsenseofcontrol,whichinturn,can
alleviatedepressivesymptoms(Staffordetal. 2011).Areas
whereresidentsknoweachother,communicate,and
interactare,infact,perceivedtofosterbettermentalhealth
amongresidents:forinstance,Torontoresidentsperceived
conceptssuchas‘‘interactionbetweenneighbors,’’
‘‘knowingneighbors,’’‘‘socialcohesion,’’and‘‘communi-
cationbetweenresidents’’tobeimportantpiecesof
neighborhoodsupportwhichwerethoughtberelatedto
positivementalhealth(Burkeetal. 2009).Indeed,Samp-
son(2013)suggeststhatresidentsinneighborhoodsofhigh
collectiveefficacy(i.e.,wheretherearesharedexpecta-
tionsaboutnormsandbehavior)arebetterabletoachieve
commongoalsandaremorelikelytoengagein(and
benefitfrom)sociallyaltruisticbehaviors.
1
Suchbenefits
areunsurprisinglyassociatedwithbettermentalhealth—
collectiveefficacy,forinstance,hasbeenfoundtoreduce
depressionamonglong-termLatinoimmigrantsinthe
UnitedStates(Vegaetal. 2011)aswellasamongolder
adults(AhernandGalea 2011),andisassociatedwithother
sociallyaltruisticbehaviorssuchasbystanderintervention
(Edwardsetal. 2014).
Itisnecessarytoaccountforneighborhooddisadvantage
whenexaminingtheimpactofneighborhoodprotective
factors,however,becausedisadvantagecanimpedethe
qualityandquantityofthehelpingmechanismswhichare
available(Goodmanetal. 2009;Sampson 2003).Inother
words,themechanismsdescribedabovethatmightmiti-
gatedepressionamongresidentsmaybelesslikelytobe
presentindisadvantagedneighborhoods.Forinstance,
scholarshavesuggestedthattheformationandextentof
socialtiesbetweenresidentsmaybelowerindisadvan-
tagedareas(seeKornhauser 1978;ShawandMcKay 1942)
potentiallybecauseoftheirfear,mistrust,andperceptions
ofdangerousness(RossandMirowsky 2009;Rossetal.
2001).Havingfeworweaksocialtiesbetweenneighbors
mightreducehelp-seekingbehaviorsamongvictimsof
partnerviolence.
Additionally,someoftheliteratureonneighborhood
protectiveeffectsondepressionandmentalhealthout-
comeshasbeenlimitedinotherways.Forinstance,data
thatisreportedbyparticipantsbutaggregatedtothe
neighborhoodlevel(e.g.,Echeverriaetal. 2008;Mairetal.
2010;Riosetal. 2012)risksconflatingoutcomeswith
predictorvariables(i.e.,neighborhoodmeasures).While
theresearchbasehasexaminedbothneighborhood-level
tiesandsupports,therehavealsobeenstudieswhichfocus
ontiesandsupportivemechanismsattheindividual-level
onlyoruseaggregatedmeasures(e.g.,Echeverriaetal.
2008;GeisandRoss 1998;Riosetal. 2012;RossandJang
2000).Further,manystudieshaveutilizedcross-sectional
designs(Mairetal. 2008),whichcannotascertainwhether
neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsprecedeincreased
depressionorwhetherpeopleseekoutsupporttohelpwith
theirdepression.Mostimportantlytothecurrentstudy,
researchhasneglectedtoexaminetheeffectsofneigh-
borhoodsupportivemechanismsontheIPV—depression
relationshipspecifically.
NeighborhoodSupportsMayMitigatetheEffect
ofPartnerViolenceonLaterDepression
WeexaminewhethertherelationshipbetweenIPVand
subsequentdepressiondependsinpartupontheneighbor-
hoodinwhichitoccurs.Basedonpreviousresearchand
theory,wemightexpectthatneighborhoodsupportive
mechanisms,suchasstrongsocialcohesion,collective
efficacy,orsocialnetworksbetweenresidentswillboth
1
However,thebeneficialeffectsofcollectiveefficacyonvarious
outcomesmaybelimitedorhinderedwhennormssuchaslegal
cynicismaretakenintoaccount(Emeryetal. 2011;KirkandMatsuda
2011).
344 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123directlydecreasedepressionandalleviatetheeffectsofIPV
ondepression,despitethelevelofdisadvantagethatexists
withinaneighborhood.Althoughwedonotdirectlytest
thesemechanisms,wesuspectthattheseneighborhood
protectivefactorswillincreasethesupportthatisavailable
fromotherstothevictim,leadingtolowerdepressionandan
attenuatedimpactofIPVonthisoutcome.Indeed,Kirstetal.
(2015)foundthatTorontoresidentswithhighperceived
socialsupportandlargersocialnetworkstodrawuponwere
lesslikelytoexperienceIPV.Wright(2012)suggestedthat
victimsofpartnerviolencemayrelyonsocialsupportfrom
othersforemotionalandfinancialhelpwhenabuseoccurs,
andthatthesesupportsystemsmayaidethevictiminleaving
theabusiverelationshiptemporarilyorpermanently.In
addition,supportivenetworkswithothersmayprovidevic-
timswithadviceregardingsafeplacestogoortheservices
thatareavailabletotheminordertoescapetheviolence
(HadeedandEl-Bassel 2006;Moe 2007;Wright 2012).In
short,neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsmayprovide
optionsforvictimstorelyonwheninneed.Second,we
believethatcommunicationwithresidentsintheneighbor-
hoodmayincreasethelikelihoodthatvictimstellothers
abouttheirvictimization(Browning 2002),andthisopen
communicationmayhelpvictimscope(Cokeretal. 2002b)
aswellasincreasethelikelihoodthatotherswilllearnabout
thevictimizationandpotentiallyintervene(Wright 2012;
WrightandBenson 2011).Havingmoreavenuesavailableto
victimsforcopingwiththeviolenceorseekinghelpto
alleviatetheabusemayreducethestresscausedbythe
victimization.Finally,supportiveneighborhoodsmay
reduceresidents’feelingsofisolationand/ormistrust,which
inturnmayreducevictims’feelingsofpowerlessness.
Weexaminethesepossibilitiesinthecurrentstudyand
focusonthreeresearchquestions:(1)whatarethelong-term
effectsofexperiencingintimatepartnerviolenceonthe
likelihoodthatfemaleslaterreportdepressivesymptoms,
whilecontrollingforotherimportantindividualandneigh-
borhoodcovariates?(2)whatarethedirecteffectsof
neighborhoodsupports(e.g.,collectiveefficacy,social
cohesion,andsocialnetworkswithothers)onfemales’
depressivesymptoms,accountingforindividualcovariates
andneighborhooddisadvantage?and(3)dotheseneigh-
borhoodsupportivemechanismsalleviatetheimpactofIPV
ondepressivesymptoms3yearslater,aftercontrollingfor
relevantindividualandneighborhoodcovariates?
Methods
Data
DataforthisstudyweredrawnfromtheProjecton
HumanDevelopmentinChicagoNeighborhoods
(PHDCN;Earlsetal. 2002),amulti-component,multi-
wavestudywhichallowsfortheexaminationofhow
neighborhoodcontextmayimpactindividual-levelout-
comes.Thisstudyreliesondatafromthreeseparate
componentsofthePHDCN:theLongitudinalCohort
Study(LCS),theCommunitySurvey,andthe1990U.S.
Census.PHDCNresearchersdividedChicago’s847cen-
sustractsinto343geographicallycontinuousneighbor-
hoodclusters(NCs).These343NCswerethenstratified
bysevencategoriesofracial/ethnicdiversityandthree
levelsofsocio-economicstatus,and80NCswerethen
selectedviaprobabilitysampling.Withinthese80NCs,
multiplecohortsofeligibleyouthandtheirprimary
caregivers(93%ofwhomwerefemale)wereselectedfor
inclusionintheLCS.Thecurrentstudyutilizesdata
collectedfromfemalecaregivers(hereafterreferredtoas
respondents)duringthefirst(1994–1997)andsecond
(1997–2000)wavesoftheLCS.
Neighborhoodmeasuresrelatedtocollectiveefficacy,
cohesion,andsocialinteractionsortiesweretakenfrom
thePHDCNCommunitySurveywhilemeasuresfor
neighborhooddisadvantagewereabstractedfromthe1990
UnitedStatesCensus.
2
TheCommunitySurvey(con-
ductedin1994–1995)sampledresidentsfromtheoriginal
343NCsandaskedabouttheirperceptionsofneighbor-
hoodsocialprocesses,includingsocialnetworksand
interactionsbetweenresidents,organizationalgroups,
values,andsoforth.Usingathree-stagesamplingdesign,
cityblocksweresampledwithineachNC,dwellingunits
weresampledwithinblocks,andoneadultresidentwas
sampledwithineachdwellingunit.Thus,participantsof
theCommunitySurveywerelargelyindependentofthose
drawnfortheLCS.
3
Thecurrentstudyincludes2959
femalerespondentslivingin80neighborhoodclusters
(hereafterreferredtoasneighborhoods)whoweremar-
ried,cohabitating,orinadatingrelationshipatwaveone
andansweredquestionsrelatedtodepressivesymptomsat
wavetwo.
4
2
StaffattheInter-universityConsortiumforPoliticalandSocial
ResearchcalculatedNC-linkedU.S.Censusmeasuresinorderto
ensuretheconfidentialityofthesubjectsofthePHDCN.
3
AlthoughtheCommunitySurveycollectedinformationfromall
343NCsaboutneighborhoodconditionsviainterviewswiththese
residents,thisstudyfocusesonlyonthose80NCsinwhichthe
participantsoftheLCSwerenested.
4
Toarriveatthe2959,atotalof292casesweredeleteddueto
missingdata.Theonlysignificantdifferencebetweenouranalysis
sampleandtheeligiblesampleoffemalecaregiversinarelationship
wasthatouranalysissamplehadslightlyfewerHispanicwomen
(p\.05).Therewerenosignificantdifferencesonthemain
independentvariableofinterestoranyothercontrolvariables.
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 345
123Measures
DepressionSymptoms
Thirteendepressionmeasureswereadaptedfromtheshort
formoftheCompositeInternationalDiagnosticInterview
(UM-CIDI;Kessleretal. 1998)andwereself-reportedby
thefemalerespondentsatwavetwo.Respondentswerefirst
askedquestionsregardingwhethertheyhadfeltsad,blue,
ordepressedfortwoormoreweeksinarowduringthe
pastyear.Ifrespondentsansweredaffirmatively,theywere
askedfollow-upquestionsregardingtheperiodoftime
theyexperienceddepressivesymptomology(i.e.,during
the2-weekperiodwhentheyfeltsadorblue,didtheylose
interestinmostthings;feeltiredorhavelowenergy;
experienceaweightchangeof10ormorepounds;have
troublefallingasleeporconcentrating;experiencefeelings
ofworthlessness;thinkaboutdeath).Respondentswere
alsoaskedthesamefollow-upquestionsregardingany
2-week(ormore)periodoftimeinthepastyearwhenthey
lostinterestinmostthingslikehobbies,work,oractivities
thattheyusuallyenjoyed.This13-itemscalehadarelia-
bilityof.902.
Topredicttheoddsofexperiencingdepressivesymp-
toms,weusedamultivariate,multilevelRaschmodel
(Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thethree-levelmodelnests
depressionitemresponseswithinpersonswithinneigh-
borhoods.Thelevel-1model(itemswithinpersons)pro-
ducesalatentvariablethatrepresentseachperson’s
susceptibilityfor depressionsymptoms (i.e.,theirlikeli-
hoodofexperiencingdepressivesymptoms).Thiscontin-
uousvariableisassumedtobenormallydistributedona
logitmetricandistheoutcomeforthelevel-two(respon-
dent-level)andlevel-three(neighborhoodlevel)models
(Osgoodetal. 2002).
IntimatePartnerViolence
SevereIPV wasassessedusingtheConflictTacticsScale
forParentsandSpouses(Straus 1979)andreflectsthe
prevalenceofseveremale-partner-perpetratedaggression
againstthefemalerespondentinthepastyear.Respondents
reportedwhethertheirmalepartnerusedanyofsixforms
ofsevereaggressionorviolenceagainsttheminthepast
year:hitwithafist,hitwithsomething,beatup,choked,
threatenedwithaknifeoragun,orusedaknifeoragun
(0 = no;1 = yes).Thismeasurereflectspartnervictim-
izationatwaveone.
NeighborhoodVariables
Drawingfrompriorresearch(Cerdaetal. 2008;Molnar
etal. 2004, 2008), concentrateddisadvantage was
operationalizedasaprincipalcomponentsfactoranalysis
includingthepercentageofresidentsinaneighborhood
whowerelivingbelowthepovertyline,receivingpublic
assistance,andunemployed(alpha = .805).Highervalues
reflectgreatereconomicdisadvantage.Becauseofthe
possibleconfoundingeffectsofdisadvantageonneigh-
borhoodsocialsupportmechanisms,concentrateddisad-
vantageislargelyincludedasacontrolmeasure.
5
Weusetwomeasuresthatreflectthedegreeoftrustand
supportbetweenneighborsinthecommunity.Following
Sampsonetal.(1997),neighborhood collectiveefficacy
wasbasedon10itemsfromadultsparticipatinginthe
CommunitySurveyandreflectsthedegreeofsocial
cohesionandinformalsocialcontrolbetweenneighbors.
Residentswereaskedhowstronglytheyagreed(onafive-
pointLikertscalerangingfrom‘‘stronglydisagree’’to
‘‘stronglyagree’’)that:peoplearoundherearewillingto
helptheirneighbors;thisisaclose-knitneighborhood;
peopleinthisneighborhoodcanbetrusted;peopleinthis
neighborhoodgenerallydon’tgetalongwitheachother
(reversecoded);andpeopleinthisneighborhooddonot
sharethesamevalues(reversecoded).Residentswerealso
askedfiveitemsregardingthelikelihood(assessedona
five-pointLikertscalefrom‘‘veryunlikely’’to‘‘very
likely’’)thattheirneighborswouldinterveneif:children
wereskippingschoolandhangingoutonastreetcorner;
childrenwerespraypaintinggraffitionalocalbuilding;
childrenwereshowingdisrespecttoanadult;afightbroke
outinfrontoftheirhouse;andthefirestationclosestto
theirhomewasthreatenedwithbudgetcuts.Following
Sampsonetal.(1997)andothers(Browningetal. 2004;
Morenoffetal. 2001),theseitemswerecombinedintoa
singlemeasureofcollectiveefficacyusingathree-level
itemresponsemodel.
6
Thelevel-onemodeladjustedthe
within-personcollectiveefficacyscoresbyitemdifficulty,
missingdata,andmeasurementerror.Thelevel-twomodel
estimatedneighborhoodcollectiveefficacyscoresadjusting
forthesocialcompositionofeachneighborhood.Inpar-
ticular,potentialbiasesinperceptionsofeachconstruct
5
Wecontrolforneighborhooddisadvantageinmultilevelanalyses
because,relativetootherstructuralconditionssuchasresidential
mobilityorethnicheterogeneity,disadvantagehasbeenfoundtobe
themostconsistentinfluenceonbothIPVanddepression(Kim 2008;
Mairetal. 2008;PinchevskyandWright 2012).
6
Itemresponsemodelingtechniquesavoidthelossofdatafrom
missingresponsestoasetofquestionsorindicators(Osgoodetal.
2002),takeitemdifficultyintoaccount(i.e.,thatsomeindicatorsof
neighborhoodconstructsmaybemoredifficultandlessprevalentthan
others),andallowsimultaneousestimationoftheimpactofindivid-
ual-levelinfluences(e.g.,age,gender)onperceptionsofthese
constructs(Sampsonetal. 2005).Theitemresponsemodelsusedin
thisstudyultimatelyprovidetheneighborhood-levelofcollective
efficacy(or,socialcohesion,orsocialinteraction)aftertheseissues
havebeenaccountedfor.
346 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123resultingfromcharacteristicsrelatedtogender,marital
status,homeownership,ethnicityandrace,residential
mobility,yearsintheneighborhood,age,andsocioeco-
nomicstatuswerecontrolledatlevel-two.Finally,the
level-threemodelallowedeachneighborhoodcluster’s
meancollectiveefficacyscoretovaryrandomlyarounda
grandmean.TheempiricalBayesresidualfromthelevel-
threemodelconstitutestheneighborhoodlevelofcollec-
tiveefficacyaftercontrollingforitemdifficultyand
neighborhoodsocialcompositionandwasthereforeusedas
the‘true’neighborhoodscoreoncollectiveefficacy.The
internalconsistencyofthisscaleattheneighborhoodlevel
was.847.
Ithasbeensuggestedthatcohesionbetweenresidents
mayuniquelyimpacttheirmentalwell-being(Stafford
etal. 2011),andKirstetal.’s(2015)findingssuggestthere
areuniquecontributionsofdifferentformsofneighbor-
hoodsocialcapital(e.g.,collectiveefficacy,perceived
socialsupport,individualnetworksize)withregardtoIPV.
Therefore,wewishedtoseparateouttheeffectsofsocial
cohesionfromsocialcontrol,asbotharecapturedinthe
overallcollectiveefficacymeasuredescribedabove.
7
We
createdaseparatemeasureof socialcohesion,apartfrom
thecollectiveefficacymeasure,andmodeledthemsepa-
ratelyintheanalyses.Socialcohesionwasbasedonfive
itemsaskedoftheCommunitySurveyparticipants;resi-
dentswereaskedhowstronglytheyagreed(from‘‘strongly
disagree’’to‘‘stronglyagree’’)that:peoplearoundhereare
willingtohelptheirneighbors;thisisaclose-knitneigh-
borhood;peopleinthisneighborhoodcanbetrusted;
peopleinthisneighborhoodgenerallydon’tgetalongwith
eachother(reversecoded);andpeopleinthisneighbor-
hooddonotsharethesamevalues(reversecoded).Inde-
pendentlyfromcollectiveefficacy,socialcohesionwas
alsooperationalizedusingathree-levelitemresponse
model.Asdescribedabove,theempiricalBayesresidual
fromthethree-levelmodelconstitutestheneighborhood
scoreonsocialcohesionaftercontrollingforsocialcohe-
sionitem-difficultyandneighborhoodsocialcomposition
(e.g.,respondentcharacteristicssuchasage,race,etc.).The
neighborhood-levelinternalconsistencyofthisscalewas
.826.
Oneitemtappingthelevelofsocialinteractionbetween
neighborswasalsoincludedinthisstudy.Adultspartici-
patingintheCommunitySurveywereaskedfourquestions
abouttheir socialinteractions withotherneighborhood
residents.Respondentswereaskedhowoften(onafour-
pointLikertscale,from‘‘never’’to‘‘often’’)theyand
peopleintheneighborhood:dofavorsforeachother;ask
eachotheradviceaboutpersonalthings;haveget-togethers
whereotherpeopleintheneighborhoodareinvited;and
visitineachother’shomesoronthestreet.Similartothe
otherneighborhoodsocialprocessesvariables,athree-level
itemresponsemodelwasusedtocreatethesocialinter-
actionsscale(seeBrowningetal. 2004).Theinternal
consistencyofthisscaleattheneighborhoodlevelwas
.734.
Finally,threemeasureswereincludedtoassessthe
existenceoffriendandfamilysocialtiesintheneighbor-
hood.ThesemeasureswerealsotakenfromtheCommu-
nitySurvey. Anyfriends and anyfamily werecomputed
basedonresidents’answerstowhethertheyhadany
friendsandanyrelativesorin-laws,respectively,livingin
theirneighborhood(excludingthosefriendsorfamily
memberswithwhomtheylived).Theseresponseswere
thenaggregatedtotheneighborhood-level.Finally,the
averagenumberoffriendsandfamily representsthe
numberoffriendsandfamily/in-lawslivingintheneigh-
borhood(eachonascaleof0–4,indicatingnoneto10or
morefriendsorfamily/in-laws)averaged,andaggregated
totheneighborhood-level.
ControlVariables
Additionalrespondent-levelfactorsdemonstratedinprior
researchtobeassociatedwiththeoddsofdepressionand
intimatepartnerviolencewerealsoincludedinanalyses
(Stithetal. 2004).Suchvariablesincludedemographic
predictors(age,race/ethnicity,married[versusdatingor
cohabitingwithapartner],householdsalary,andemploy-
mentstatus),opportunityvariables(isolation),andprior
lifehistories(stressfullifeevent,priordepression).
Importantly,priordepression(ayes/noindicatorself-re-
portedbytherespondent)wasassessedatwaveone,while
thedepressivesymptomsoutcomewasassessedatwave
two,whichallowedustocontrolforthetemporalordering
betweencurrentandpriordepression.Table 1 provides
additionalinformationaboutthesecontrolvariables.
StatisticalAnalyses
FollowingRaudenbushetal.(2003),Sampsonetal.(2005),
andothers(ZimmermanandMessner 2010, 2011),we
7
Thesamefiveitemsthatmeasuresocialcohesionarealsoincluded
inthecollectiveefficacymeasure.Webelievethisoverlapis
conceptuallytolerableforthepurposesofourinquiry.First,wewere
interestedintheeffectofcollectiveefficacyonbothdepressionand
theIPV—depressionrelationship,andthus,neededtoincludethe
measureofcollectiveefficacyasithasbeenexaminedinprior
research(e.g.,Sampsonetal. 1997).Additionally,therehasbeen
somerecentattentiontotheimportanceofsocialcohesionwithregard
todepression(Mairetal. 2010)aswellasbyitselfasafacilitatorof
positiveneighborhoodbehavior(e.g.,informalsocialcontrol,see
Warner 2014).Wewereinterestedinitsuniqueeffects—apartfrom
collectiveefficacy—andthereforechosetoincludeaseparate
measureofsocialcohesioninouranalyses.Collinearitydidnot
presentaproblem,aswemodeledcollectiveefficacyandsocial
cohesionseparately.
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 347
123employamultilevellogisticregressionmodeltopredictthe
oddsthatarespondentlivinginagivenneighborhoodwill
reportexperiencingdepressivesymptoms.Thisallowsus
toutilizeall34,766responsestothewavetwodepression
symptomitemsprovidedbyall2959ofthefemalesliving
in80neighborhoodsinoursample.Thus,ouranalytic
techniqueincludesanyfemalewhorespondedtoatleast
onedepressionsymptomitemaskedatwavetwo.This
techniqueeffectivelyavoidsthelossofdataduetomissing
itemresponses(Osgoodetal. 2002)andtakesitemdiffi-
cultyintoaccount(i.e.,somedepressionsymptomsareless
prevalentthanothers)(Sampsonetal. 2005).
ThemultivariatemultilevelRaschmodelisathree-level
modelinwhichdichotomousdepressionitemsarenested
withinpersons,whoarenestedwithinneighborhoods
(Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thelevel-oneoutcomeisthe
log-oddsofrespondingaffirmativelytoitem i of m - 1
depressionitems,by j person,livingin k neighborhood.
Table1 Descriptiveinformationfordependentandindependentvariables
MeanSDMin–
Max
Dependentvariable
DepressionSymptomsThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon13indicatorsofdepression(wave2).09.290to1
Individual-levelvariables
SevereIPVSixitemmeasureindicatingwhetherthemalepartnerusedsevereaggressionagainstthe
femalerespondentinthepastyear(hitwithafist,hitwithsomething,beatup,choked,
threatenedwithaknifeorgun,orusedaknifeorgun)(wave1)
.15.350to1
AgeAgeoffemalerespondent(wave1)32.727.9015.02to
80.93
HispanicFemalerespondentisHispanic(wave1).48.500to1
AfricanAmericanFemalerespondentisAfricanAmerican(wave1).31.460to1
OtherraceFemalerespondentisanotherrace/ethnicity(wave1).04.190to1
Caucasian(ref)FemalerespondentisCaucasian(wave1).16.370to1
MarriedFemalerespondentismarried(wave1).63.480to1
HouseholdsalaryMaximumhouseholdsalaryrangingfromlessthan$5000toover$50,000(wave1)4.011.951to7
UnemploymentFemalerespondentisunemployed(wave1).48.500to1
IsolationFive-itemscalereportedbythefemaleindicatinghersocialisolation(feelingalone,not
havingfriendstotalkto,eachonascaleof1-3,indicatingverytruetonottrue)(wave1)
(reliability = .613)
-.001.00 -.92to
3.31
StressfullifeeventFemales’exposuretostressfullifeeventswherebyafriend,familymember,or
acquaintancewashurt(e.g.,shotatorraped)orkilledbyaviolentact(waves1and2)
.48.500to1
PriordepressionFemalerespondentreportedpriordepression(wave1).12.330to1
Neighborhood-levelvariables
Concentrated
disadvantage
Principalcomponentsfactoranalysisusingthreeitems(reliability = .805)fromthe1990
Census:thepercentageofresidentsbelowpoverty,householdsreceivingpublic
assistance,andresidentsunemployed
.001.00 -1.51to
2.35
CollectiveefficacyThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon10indicatorsofsocialcohesionandinformal
socialcontrolreportedbyadultresidentsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .847)
-.01.22 -.46to
.64
SocialcohesionThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon5indicatorsofsocialcohesionandtrustbetween
neighborsreportedbyadultresidentsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .826)
-.00.20 -.46to
.64
SocialinteractionsThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon4indicatorsofinteractionsbetween
neighborhoodsreportedbyadultsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .734)
-.00.17 -.52to
.63
AnyfriendsWhetheradultsintheCommunitySurveyreportedhavinganyfriendslivingintheir
neighborhood.ResponseswereaggregatedtotheNC-level
.83.08.62–.98
AnyfamilyWhetheradultsintheCommunitySurveyreportedhavinganyrelativesorin-lawslivingin
theirneighborhood.ResponseswereaggregatedtotheNC-level
.45.170–.86
Averagenumberof
familyandfriends
Numberofrelativesorin-lawsandfriendslivingintheneighborhoodasreportedbythe
adultsintheCommunitySurvey(eachonascaleof0–4,indicating0–10ormore).
ThesemeasureswereaveragedandthenaggregatedtotheNC-level
1.52.35.96–3.02
Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters
348 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123Thisvariablelocatesitemseveritiesonthelogitscale
(Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thus,thelevel-onemodel
adjuststhewithin-personpropensityfordepressivesymp-
tomsbyitemseverity,missingdata,andmeasurement
error.Thelevel-oneinterceptservesastheoutcomeforthe
level-twoandlevel-threemodelsandisassumedtobe
normallydistributedonalogitscale.
Thelevel-twomodelexaminestheeffectsofperson-
levelcorrelates(e.g.,age,maritalstatus,severeIPV)onthe
level-oneintercept(susceptibilityfordepressivesymp-
toms),whilealsocontrollingforitemseveritiesatlevel-
one.Alloftheperson-leveleffectsweregrand-meancen-
tered.Fortheneighborhooddirecteffectsmodels,the
effectsofallcovariateswereassumedtobefixedacross
neighborhoods;however,theanalysesexaminingthecross-
levelinteractionsallowedtheeffectofexperiencingsevere
IPVtovaryacrossneighborhoods(p\.05).
Thelevel-threemodelallowsestimationofthesuscep-
tibilityofdepressivesymptomsacrossneighborhoods.The
level-threeintercepts-as-outcomesmodelexaminesthe
directeffectsofneighborhooddisadvantageandsupportive
mechanismsonthelevel-twointercept(i.e.,thesuscepti-
bilityofexperiencingdepressionsymptoms,controllingfor
person-levelcorrelatesatlevel-twoanditemseveritiesat
level-one).Thelevel-threeslopes-and-intercepts-as-out-
comesmodelallowsforexaminationofthecross-level
interactionbetweenneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms
andthelevel-tworelationshipbetweenexperiencingsevere
IPVandsusceptibilityforexperiencingdepressionsymp-
toms,whileadjustingfortheindividualandneighborhood
maineffects.Multicollinearitywasnotaprobleminanyof
themodels(tolerancevalueswereabove.40).
Results
Tables 2 and 3 presenttheresultsofourthreeresearch
questions.Table 2 presentsthemaineffectofexperiencing
severeIPVfromone’spartneratwaveoneondepressive
symptomsapproximately3yearslater,controllingfor
neighborhoodvariablesaswellasindividual-levelfactors,
suchaspriordepressionandmaritalstatus.Theseanalyses
donotincludethecross-levelinteractionbetweenneigh-
borhoodsupportivemechanismsandtheIPV—depression
relationship.Theresultsoftheindividual-levelmodels
suggestthat,asexpected,experiencingsevereabusefrom
one’spartnersignificantlyincreasesthelikelihoodthat
femaleswillreportsubsequentdepressivesymptoms;
importantly,thiseffectissignificantevenaftercontrolling
fortheimpactofpriordepression.Additionally,struggling
withpriordepression(comparedtonotreportingprior
depressivesymptoms),experiencingastressfullifeevent
(versusnotexperiencingsuchevents),andhavinghigher
householdsalaries(asopposedtolowersalaries)increased
thelikelihoodofexperiencingdepressivesymptomsacross
allmodels.Beingmarried(asopposedtodatingor
cohabitingwithapartner)protectedwomenagainstexpe-
riencingdepressivesymptomsacrossallmodels.
InthebottomhalfofTable 2,weprovidetheneigh-
borhooddirecteffectsondepressivesymptoms,controlling
forthemaineffectofneighborhooddisadvantageand
respondent-compositionalfactorsinallanalyses.Resultsof
theneighborhooddirecteffectsindicatethatmanyofthe
neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsare,infact,protec-
tiveagainstexperiencingdepressivesymptoms,evenwhen
neighborhooddisadvantageandindividual-levelprior
depressionaretakenintoaccount.Specifically,women
whoresideinneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigherlevels
ofcollectiveefficacy(Model1),socialcohesion(Model2),
socialinteractionsbetweenresidents(Model3),and
neighborhoodsinwhichmanypeoplehadfriendsand
familymembers(Model6)werelesslikelytoreport
experiencingdepressivesymptomsthanwomenresidingin
neighborhoodscharacterizedbylesscollectiveefficacy,
cohesion,networks,andfewerfriendsorfamilymembers
onaverage.Neighborhoodconcentrateddisadvantagewas
alsosignificantly(thoughmodestly)associatedwitha
lowerlikelihoodofdepressivesymptomswhencollective
efficacy(Model1)andsocialcohesion(Model2)werealso
includedintheanalyses,thoughitwasnotdirectlyrelated
todepressivesymptomsinanyoftheothermodels.
InTable 3,weincludethecross-levelinteractions
betweenneighborhoodsupportsandtheIPV—subsequent
depressionrelationship.Thesearereportedintherows
belowtheSevereIPVvariable.Theseinteractionsare
assessedsimultaneouslywiththeindividualandneigh-
borhoodmaineffects.Nosubstantivechangesintheindi-
vidualorneighborhoodmaineffectswereobservedwhen
thecross-levelinteractionwasincludedinTable 3.As
mentionedabove,theimpactofexperiencingsevereIPV
fromone’spartnerinfactvariedacrossneighborhoods,and
wasallowedtovaryforthecross-levelinteractionmod-
els(asindicatedbytheitalicizedcoefficients).Thepurpose
ofexaminingthecross-levelinteractionswastodetermine
ifneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,suchascollective
efficacy,explainedthisvariation,perhapsbymitigatingthe
impactofIPVondepressivesymptoms.Wefoundvery
littleevidencethatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms
alleviatedtheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepression,and
theonlytwosignificantmoderatingeffectswefoundwere
somewhatcounterintuitive.Model5inTable 3 indicates
thattheimpactofexperiencingsevereIPVonlater
depressivesymptomswassignificantlystrongerforwomen
whoresidedinneighborhoodscharacterizedbygreater
proportionsoffamilymembersintheneighborhood(as
reportedbyrespondentsoftheCommunitySurvey,notthe
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 349
123respondentsofIPV),whileModel6demonstratesthatthe
influenceofIPVondepressivesymptomswasalsostronger
forwomenlivinginneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigher
averagenumbersoffamilyandfriendslivinginthesame
neighborhood(thoughthiseffectwasmodest,reaching
significanceatthe p B .10level).
Figure 1 depictsthecross-levelinteractiondisplayedin
Model5ofTable 3,whileFig. 2 depictsthecross-level
interactioninModel6.Asshown,therelationshipbetween
IPVandthelikelihoodofexperiencingdepressivesymp-
tomswassteepandpositiveinneighborhoodscharacter-
izedbyhigh(onestandarddeviationabovethemean)
familyassociates(Fig. 1)andfriendsandfamilymembers
(Fig. 2)intheneighborhood.TheimpactofIPVon
depressivesymptomsweakened(asindicatedbytheslope
tiltinginthenegativedirection)inareascharacterizedby
low(onestandarddeviationbelowthemean)family
members(Fig. 1)andfamilyandfriends(Fig. 2).Thus,
althoughweexpectedthatneighborhoodsupportive
mechanisms,suchastieswithothers,wouldpotentially
shieldvictimsfromexperiencingdepressivesymptoms,we
foundtheoppositeeffect–insomeneighborhoods,par-
ticularlythosecharacterizedbyhighlevelsoffamily
members,theimpactofbeingavictimofpartnerviolence
onlaterdepressivesymptomswasactuallyworse.
Discussion
Manyscholarsagreethatneighborhoodcontextisimpor-
tantwhenitcomestobothintimatepartnerviolenceand
depression(orothermentalhealthproblems)(e.g.,Benson
etal. 2003;Goodmanetal. 2009;KimandRoss 2009),and
anumberofstudieshaveexaminedthenegativeordetri-
mentalimpactsofneighborhoodswithregardtothese
outcomes(e.g.,Mairetal. 2008;Miles-Doan 1998).
Table2 Directeffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceandneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsondepressionsymptoms
Model1Model2Model3Model4Model5Model6
b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)
Intercept -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06)
Individual-leveleffects
Age.00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01)
Hispanic.25(.15).24(.16).26(.15).30*(.15).32*(.15).30*(.15)
AfricanAmerican.06(.21).08(.21).05(.21).05(.21).07(.21).03(.22)
Otherrace.07(.28).08(.29).07(.28).08(.28).07(.27).06(.28)
Married -.31*
(.13) -.30*(.13) -.31*(.13) -.32*(.13) -.32*(.13) -.32*(.13)
Householdsalary.07*
(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03)
Unemployment.09(.14).09(.14).10(.14).09(.14).08(.14).09(.14)
Isolation.06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).07(.07).06(.07)
Stressfullifeevent.38**
(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11)
Priordepression1.07**
(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)
SevereIPV.50**
(.15).50**(.16).51**(.16).50**(.15).50**(.15).50**(.15)
Neighborhooddirecteffects
Concentrateddisadvantage -.15 (.08) -.16*(.07) -.07(.08) -.09(.08) -.07(.08) -.07(.08)
Collectiveefficacy -.59*
(.24)––––––––––
Socialcohesion–– -.93**(.31)––––––––
Socialinteraction–––– -.61*(.28)––––––
Anyfriends–––––– -.75(.60)––––
Anyfamily–––––––– -.25(.31)––
Averagenumberoffamilyand
friends
–––––––––– -.23*(.11)
Variancecomponents
Individuallevelintercept7.7947.8037.7997.7947.7907.793
Neighborhoodintercept.205.173.195.207.215.206
Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters
** p B .01;* p B .05;
p B .10
350 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123However,lessattentionhasbeenpaidtothepotential
protectiveorhealth-promotingeffectsthatsometypesof
neighborhoodsprovidefortheirresidents(forexceptions,
seeforinstance,AhernandGalea 2011;Kim 2010;Kim
andRoss 2009;Mairetal. 2008, 2010;RossandJang
2000;Staffordetal. 2011).Additionally,theinterplayof
partnerviolence,neighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,
anddepressivesymptoms,toourknowledge,hasnotbeen
assessed,limitingourunderstandingforhowtheIPV—
depressionrelationshipoperateswhenneighborhoodcon-
textisconsidered.Ourstudysoughttoadvancethisliter-
aturebyexploringwhetherneighborhoodsupportive
mechanismsalleviatedthedetrimentalimpactofIPVon
subsequentdepressivesymptoms.Ourresultssuggestthat
theimpactofviolentrelationshipsonlatermentalhealth
outcomesmaydependinpartonneighborhoodcharacter-
istics,particularlysocialties,butcontrarytoourexpecta-
tions,thepresenceofhighlevelsofneighborhoodties
Table3 Effectsofintimatepartnerviolenceandneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsondepressionsymptoms,andcross-levelinteractions
betweenneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsandtheIPV—depressionrelationship
Model1Model2Model3Model4Model5Model6
b(SE)b(SE)B(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)
Intercept -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.85**(.06)
Individual-leveleffectsandcross-levelinteractions
Age.00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01)
Hispanic.22(.15).21(.15).23(.14).27(.15).30*(.14).27(.14)
AfricanAmerican.06(.20).08(.20).06(.21).05(.21).08(.21).05(.21)
Otherrace.01(.28).02(.28) -.01(.27).02(.28) -.02(.26) -.03(.27)
Married -.33*
(.13) -.32*(.13) -.33*(.13) -.34*(.13) -.34**(.13) -.34**(.13)
Householdsalary.07*
(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03)
Unemployment.08(.14).09(.14).10(.14).08(.14).08(.14).09(.14)
Isolation.06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07)
Stressfullifeevent.37**
(.11).37**(.11).37**(.11).38**(.11).37**(.11).37**(.11)
Priordepression1.09**
(.17)1.09**(.17)1.09**(.17)1.10**(.17)1.10**(.17)1.09**(.17)
SevereIPV .50**
(.16) .50** (.17) .51** (.16) .48** (.16) .44** (.15) .48** (.16)
9 Collectiveefficacy.03(.81)––––––––––
9 Socialcohesion––.24(1.04)––––––––
9 Socialinteraction––––1.02(.79)––––––
9 Anyfriends–––––– -1.14(1.64)––––
9 Anyfamily––––––––1.91*(.91)––
9 Averagenumberoffamily
andfriends
––––––––––.63
(.34)
Neighborhoodmaineffects
Concentrateddisadvantage -.16
(.08) -.16*(.07) -.07(.08) -.10(.08) -.07(.08) -.07(.08)
Collectiveefficacy -.60*(.24)––––––––––
Socialcohesion–– -.92**(.32)––––––––
Socialinteraction–––– -.65*(.28)––––––
Anyfriends–––––– -.74(.62)––––
Anyfamily–––––––– -.25(.31)––
Averagenumberoffamilyand
friends
–––––––––– -.22*(.11)
Variancecomponents
Individual-levelintercept7.6137.6227.6227.6167.6187.620
Neighborhoodintercept.207.175.202.211.220.214
SevereIPVintercept1.8911.8721.8181.8281.6611.729
Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters
Thesignificanceofitalicvalueindicatesrandomlyvaryingcoefficient
** p B .01;* p B .05;
p B .10;
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 351
123appearedtostrengthenratherthanattenuatetherelation-
shipbetweenIPVanddepressivesymptoms.Wedidfind,
however,thatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,par-
ticularlythoserelatedtocollectiveefficacy,cohesion,and
socialinteractionswithneighbors,alleviatedthelikelihood
ofexperiencingsymptomsofdepressionevenaftercon-
trollingfortheeffectsofpriordepression.Wediscussour
findingsbelow.
Ourfirstgoalwastoexaminethelong-termimpactof
IPVondepressivesymptomsusinglongitudinaldata,as
fewstudieshaveassessedtheprospectiveimpactofviolent
relationshipsonsuchanoutcome(Campbell 2002;Fletcher
2010;Johnsonetal. 2014).Wefoundthatexperiencing
severeIPVsignificantlyincreasedthelikelihoodthat
womenreporteddepressivesymptomsyearslater.The
relationshipbetweenIPVanddepressionwasrobust,
maintainingasignificanteffectdespitehighlyspecified
modelswhichincludedseveralotherimportantcovariates,
suchaspriordepressionand,perhapsevenmoreimpor-
tantly,whilesimultaneouslyinvestigatingcontextual
influences(Goodmanetal. 2009).Giventhestrengthofour
multilevellongitudinalmodels,ourresultsdemonstratethat
IPVisasignificantriskfactorforsubsequentmentalhealth
problemsthatisindependentfromtheeffectsofother
predictorsofdepression,suchaspriordepression,stressful
lifeevents,andisolation.ThatIPVmaintainedastrong
longitudinaleffectondepressioninthesemodelssuggests
thatitremainsacentralpublichealthconcern(seealso
Bonomietal. 2006;CaetanoandCunradi 2003;DeMaris
andKaukinen 2005;Golding 1999).
-4
-3
-2
-1
0
1
High Proporon of
Family Members in
Neighborhood
Average Proporon of
Family Members in
Neighborhood
Low Proporon of
Family Members in
Neighborhood
Likelihood of Depression Symptoms
Severe Inmate Partner Violence
0 1
Fig.1 Therelationship
betweensevereintimatepartner
violenceanddepression
symptomsbyneighborhood
proportionoffamilyties.
Controlsforneighborhood
concentrateddisadvantageand
allrespondent-levelcovariates
-3
-2
-1
0
High Levels of Family and
Friends in Neighborhood
Average Levels of Family
and Friends in
Neighborhood
Low Levels of Family and
Friends in Neighborhood
Likelihood of Depression Symptoms
Severe Inmate Partner Violence
0
1
Fig.2 Therelationship
betweensevereintimatepartner
violenceanddepression
symptomsbyaveragenumber
offamilyandfriendsin
neighborhood.Controlsfor
concentrateddisadvantageand
allrespondent-levelcovariates
352 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123Ournextgoalsweretoexamineseveralneighborhood-
levelprotectivefactors—particularlythosemeasuring
socialconnectionsandcohesivenessamongneighbors—
whichhavebeensuggestedasmechanismsthatmight
alleviatementalhealthsymptomsand/ortheeffectsof
abuse(AhernandGalea 2011;Burkeetal. 2006;Mairetal.
2008;O’Campoetal. 2005;Staffordetal. 2011).We
soughttoexamineboththedirectandmoderatinginflu-
encesoftheseneighborhoodcharacteristicswhilecontrol-
lingforimportantindividual-levelcovariates.Consistent
withpriorresearchreportingadirectinfluenceofneigh-
borhoodsondepression(e.g.,Kim 2010;Mairetal. 2008),
wefoundthatneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsdo
appeartoprotectindividualsfromexperiencingsymptoms
ofdepression.Wesuspectthatneighborhoodfactorssuch
ascollectiveefficacy,socialcohesionbetweenneighbors,
socialinteractionsamongresidents,andhavingmore
familyandfriendswhoresideinone’sneighborhoodare
protectiveagainstnegativementalhealthsymptoms
becausetheyprovidesupport,increasecommunication,
andlowerfeelingsofisolation(e.g.,Browning 2002;
Sampson 2013;Vegaetal. 2011).Assourcesofsupport,
thesetiesmightalsoreducetheeffectsofstress,increase
feelingsofcontrol,fosteraltruisticbehaviors,orreduce
feelingsoffearormistrust,allofwhichinturnmight
alleviatefeelingsofdepression(AhernandGalea 2011;
KimandRoss 2009;RossandJang 2000;Sampson 2013;
Staffordetal. 2011).
However,wealsofoundthatsomeneighborhoodsup-
portivemechanismsarenotnecessarilyprotectiveagainst
depression,particularlywhenpartnerviolenceiscon-
cerned.Livinginneighborhoodswheregreaterproportions
offamilymembers(relativesorin-laws)residedactually
exacerbatedthenegativeimpactofIPVondepressive
symptomsamongfemales.Likewise,wefoundthatwomen
wholivedinneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigherpro-
portionsoffamilyandfriendtiesweremoreat-riskfor
depressivesymptomsfollowingIPVthanwomenwhose
neighborhoodswerecharacterizedbyfewersuch‘‘sup-
ports.’’Whilethe‘‘family’’elementmaybelargelydriving
thislastfinding,westillconsiderbotheffectsnoteworthy
andunexpected.Althoughneighborhoodsocialties—par-
ticularlybetweenfamilyand/orfriends—islargelydeemed
aprotectivemechanismformanyoutcomes(e.g.,Bellair
1997;WellmanandWortley 1990),includingbothpartner
violence(e.g.,Agoffetal. 2007;Wright 2012)and
depression(e.g.,KimandRoss 2009),wefoundthe
oppositetobetrue,atleastregardingtheimpactofIPVon
depressivesymptoms.Afewpossibilitiesmayaccountfor
thesecontradictoryfindings.RecallthattheCommunity
Surveysampledrespondentsfromneighborhoodswho
werenotnecessarilyparticipantsoftheLCS—thus,
‘‘family’’tiesintheneighborhoodlikelydonotreflectthe
victim’sownfamilymembers—theysimplyreflectthat
suchtiesexistwithintheneighborhood.Inthiscase,
womenwhoarebeingvictimizedmaynothavetheirown
familymemberslivingcloseby.Indeed,itispossiblethat
theIPVvictimsintheseneighborhoodsactuallymayhave
feltdoublyisolatediftheyhadnolocalfamilyorfriends
whileeveryonearoundthemwasstronglytiedintoalocal
familial(orfriendship)network.Wesoughttotakethis
possibilityintoaccount,however,byincludingthe
females’levelofisolationasanindividual-levelcontrol.
Thatitwasnotsignificantinourmodelssuggestsanother
explanationiswarranted.Forinstance,wedonotknowif
thevictimizedfemaleactuallyutilizedthesetiesforhelp.
Whilesome(Browning 2002;VanWyketal. 2003;Wright
andBenson 2011)havesuggestedthatmoresocialtiesmay
makeitmorelikelythattheviolencewillbecomepublic
knowledge,itmaybethatthesetiesweresimplynot
activatedorsoughtoutforhelp(WrightandBenson 2011).
SinceIPVisoftentimesconsideredveryprivateinnature
(Strausetal. 2006),thisiscertainlyapossibility.Itisalso
possiblethatbeingsurroundedbymorefamilyties(evenif
notone’sownfamilymembers)inone’sneighborhoodputs
pressureonwomentostayintherelationship,especiallyif
familiesintheareadonotcondonedivorceorseparation,
evenfromaviolentpartner(Agoffetal. 2007).Alterna-
tively,itispossiblethatinthecontextofpartnerviolence,
somesocialsupportsarenothelpful,andmayinfactbe
harmfulbysupportingorcondoningtheuseofviolence
withinrelationships.Weareunabletoexaminewhetherthe
neighborhoodtiesmeasuredherearesupportiveofIPV,
andthereforecannottestthispotentialexplanation.Ifthe
socialtiesintheneighborhoodswerebetweenresidents
whoweresupportiveofviolenceinrelationships,thiscould
explaintheexacerbatingeffectfoundbetweenneighbor-
hoodsocialtiesandtheimpactofIPVonlaterdepression.
Wecannotbesurewhichexplanation,ifany,iscorrect
giventheavailabledata,butweencouragefutureresearch
toattempttodisentanglethecomplexrelationshipthat
appearstoexistbetweenneighborhood-levelsocialties
(especiallywithfamilyorrelatives)andpartnerviolence.
Asidefromtheeffectoffamilyandfriends,wefoundno
otherevidencethatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms
(collectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,socialinteraction)
moderatedtheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepression.
Perhapstheneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsexam-
inedherearetoogeneraltoalleviatetheeffectsofIPVon
mentalhealth—itispossiblethatotherneighborhood
constructsgearedmorespecificallytoviolenceandmental
healthsupport,intervention,and/orprevention(e.g.,
counselingservices,shelters,etc.)wouldbemorerelevant.
Futureresearchshouldconsiderthepotentialforsuch
neighborhoodsupportivemechanismstoeasetheeffectsof
specificviolencestressorssuchasIPVandabuseon
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 353
123negativementalhealthoutcomessothataggregatepre-
ventionandinterventionscanbeimplemented(seealso
Cunradi 2010).
Limitations
Unfortunately,ourstudyisnotwithoutlimitations.We
reliedsolelyondatafromfemalecaregiversanddidnot
includemalesaspotentialvictimsofIPV.Therefore,wedo
notknowifourfindingswouldgeneralizetofemales’
violenceperpetratedagainstmales.Similarly,ourstudy
doesnotexaminedatingviolencebyadolescents.Whilewe
examinedtheimpactoftheprevalenceofsevereIPVon
females’depression,wedidnotexaminetheimpactofthe
frequencyorchronicityofsuchviolence.Itispossiblethat
morefrequentormoresevereviolence,whileperhapsmore
detrimentaltodepression(Campbell 2002),couldbe
impactedbyneighborhoodfactorsdifferently.Finally,our
datawerecollectedfromwomeninonecity(Chicago)
duringthemid-1990sandintotheearly2000s;although
thePHDCNisahighlyrespecteddataset,wecannotensure
thatourresultswouldgeneralizetootherpopulationsor
timeperiods.
Ourstudyaddstoburgeoningevidencethatneighbor-
hoodcontextimpactsvariousinterpersonaloutcomes,in
bothdirectandmoderatingways.Itappearsthatneigh-
borhoodsupportivemechanismsmaydirectlyprotect
femalesfromnegativementalhealthproblemssuchas
experiencingdepressivesymptoms.Theseneighborhood
supportivemechanisms,thoughlessoftenconsidered,are
importanttoexamineforpreventionandinterventionpur-
poses,andsuggestthatjustasneighborhoodscanbe
detrimental,theycanalsobepositiveandprotective.
Neighborhoodcollectiveefficacy,cohesion,andinteraction
betweenneighborsmightincreasefeelingsofsupport,
belongingness,investment,communication,andother
positiveattributes,whichmayinturnreducedepressive
symptoms.Webelieveitisimportantforresearchersto
continuetoexaminethepositiveandpotentiallymitigating
influencesofneighborhoods,aswellastheirdetrimental
effects,inordertobetterunderstandforwhomandunder
whichcircumstancesviolentrelationshipsandmental
healthareinfluencedbycontextualfactors.
References
Agoff,C.,Herrera,C.,&Castro,R.(2007).Theweaknessoffamily
tiesandtheirperpetuatingeffectsongenderviolence:A
qualitativestudyinMexico. ViolenceAgainstWomen,13(11),
1206–1220.
Ahern,J.,&Galea,S.(2011).Collectiveefficacyandmajor
depressioninurbanneighborhoods. AmericanJournalof
Epidemiology,173(12),1453–1462.
Aneshensel,C.S.,&Sucoff,C.A.(1996).Theneighborhoodcontext
ofadolescentmentalhealth. JournalofHealthandSocial
Behavior,37(4),293–310.
Bellair,P.E.(1997).Socialinteractionandcommunitycrime:
Examiningtheimportanceofneighbornetworks. Criminology,
35(4),677–704.
Benson,M.L.,Fox,G.L.,DeMaris,A.,&VanWyk,J.(2003).
Neighborhooddisadvantage,individualeconomicdistressand
violenceagainstwomeninintimaterelationships. Journalof
QuantitativeCriminology,19(3),207–235.
Bonomi,A.E.,Thompson,R.S.,Anderson,M.,Reid,R.J.,Carrell,
D.,Dimer,J.A.,&Rivara,F.P.(2006).Intimatepartner
violenceandwomen’sphysical,mental,andsocialfunctioning.
AmericanJournalofPreventiveMedicine,30(6),458–466.
Browning,C.R.(2002).Thespanofcollectiveefficacy:Extending
socialdisorganizationtheorytopartnerviolence. Journalof
MarriageandFamily,64(4),833–850.
Browning,C.R.,Feinberg,S.L.,&Dietz,R.D.(2004).Theparadox
ofsocialorganization:Networks,collectiveefficacy,andviolent
crimeinurbanneighborhoods. SocialForces,83(2),503–534.
Burke,J.,O’Campo,P.,&Peak,G.L.(2006).Neighborhood
influencesandintimatepartnerviolence:Doesgeographic
settingmatter? JournalofUrbanHealth,83(2),182–194.
Burke,J.,O’Campo,P.,Salmon,C.,&Walker,R.(2009).Pathways
connectingneighborhoodinfluencesandmentalwell-being:
Socioeconomicpositionandgenderdifferences. SocialScience
andMedicine,68,1294–1304.
Caetano,R.,&Cunradi,C.(2003).Intimatepartnerviolenceand
depressionamongWhites,Blacks,andHispanics. Annalsof
Epidemiology,13,661–665.
Caldwell,J.E.,Swan,S.C.,&Woodbrown,V.D.(2012).Gender
differencesinintimatepartnerviolenceoutcomes. Psychologyof
Violence,2(1),42–57.
Campbell,J.C.(2002).Healthconsequencesofintimatepartner
violence. Lancet,359(9341),1331–1336.
Campbell,J.C.,Kub,J.E.,&Rose,L.(1996).Depressioninbattered
women. JournaloftheAmericanMedicalWomen’sAssociation,
51(3),106–110.
Cerda,M.,Sanchez,B.N.,Galea,S.,Tracy,M.,&Buka,S.L.
(2008).Estimatingco-occuringbehavioraltrajectorieswithina
neighborhoodcontext:Acasestudyofmultivariatetransition
modelsforclustereddata. AmericanJournalofEpidemiology,
168(10),1190–1203.
Coker,A.L.,Davis,K.E.,Arias,I.,Desai,R.A.,Sanderson,M.,
Brandt,H.M.,&Smith,P.H.(2002a).Physicalandmental
healtheffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceformenandwomen.
AmericanJournalofPreventiveMedicine,23(4),260–268.
Coker,A.L.,Smith,P.H.,Thompson,M.P.,McKeown,R.E.,
Bethea,L.,&Davis,K.E.(2002b).Socialsupportprotects
againstthenegativeeffectsofpartnerviolenceonmentalhealth.
JournalofWomen’sHealth&Gender-BasedMedicine,11(5),
465–476.
Cunradi,C.(2010).Neighborhoods,alcoholoutletsandintimate
partnerviolence:Addressingresearchgapsinexplanatory
mechanisms. InternationalJournalofEnvironmentalResearch
andPublicHealth,7,799–813.
DeMaris,A.,&Kaukinen,C.(2005).Violentvictimizationand
women’smentalandphysicalhealth:Evidencefromanational
sample. JournalofResearchinCrimeandDelinquency,42(4),
384–411.
Earls,F.J.,Brooks-Gunn,J.,Raudenbush,S.W.,&Sampson,R.J.
(2002). ProjectonhumandevelopmentinChicago
354 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123neighborhoods(PHDCN).AnnArbor,MI:Inter-university
CorsortiumforPoliticalandSocialResearch.
Echeverria,S.,DiezRoux,A.V.,Shea,S.,Borrell,L.N.,&Jackson,
S.(2008).Associationsofneighborhoodproblemsandneigh-
borhoodsocialcohesionwithmentalhealthandhealthbehav-
iors:Themulti-ethnicstudyofatherosclerosis. Health&Place,
14(4),853–865.
Edwards,K.M.,Mattingly,M.J.,Dixon,K.J.,&Banyard,V.L.
(2014).Communitymatters:Intimatepartnerviolenceamong
ruralyoungadults. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,
53,198–207.
Emery,C.R.,Jolley,J.M.,&Wu,S.(2011).Desistancefrom
intimatepartnerviolence:Theroleoflegalcynicism,collective
efficacy,andsocialdisorganizationinChicagoneighborhoods.
AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,48,373–383.
Fletcher,J.(2010).Theeffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceonhealth
inyoungadulthoodintheUnitedStates. SocialScienceand
Medicine,70,130–135.
Geis,K.J.,&Ross,C.E.(1998).Anewlookaturbanalienation:The
effectofneighborhooddisorderonperceivedpowerlessness.
SocialPsychologyQuarterly,61(3),232–246.
Golding,J.M.(1999).Intimatepartnerviolenceasariskfactorfor
mentaldisorders:Ameta-analysis. JournalofFamilyViolence,
14(2),99–132.
Goodman,L.A.,Smyth,K.F.,Borges,A.M.,&Singer,R.(2009).
Whencrisescollide:Howintimatepartnerviolenceandpoverty
intersecttoshapewomen’smentalhealthandcoping? Trauma,
Violence,&Abuse,10(4),306–329.
Hadeed,L.F.,&El-Bassel,N.(2006).SocialsupportamongAfro-
Trinidadianwomenexperiencingintimatepartnerviolence.
ViolenceAgainstWomen,12(8),740–760.
Hill,T.D.,Ross,C.E.,&Angel,R.J.(2005).Neighborhood
disorder,psychophysiologicaldistress,andhealth. Journalof
HealthandSocialBehavior,46(2),170–186.
Johnson,W.L.,Giordano,P.C.,Longmore,M.A.,&Manning,W.
D.(2014).Intimatepartnerviolenceanddepressivesymptoms
duringadolescenceandyoungadulthood. JournalofHealthand
SocialBehavior,55(1),39–55.
Kessler,R.C.,Andrews,G.,Mroczerk,D.,Ustun,B.,&Wittchen,
H.-U.(1998).TheWorldHealthOrganizationcompositeinter-
nationaldiagnosticinterviewshort-form(CIDI-SF). Interna-
tionalJournalofMethodsinPsychiatricResearch,7(4),
171–185.
Kim,D.(2008).Bluesfromtheneighborhood?Neighborhood
characteristicsanddepression. EpidemiologicReviews,30,
101–117.
Kim,J.(2010).Neighborhooddisadvantageandmentalhealth:The
roleofneighborhooddisorderandsocialrelationships. Social
ScienceResearch,39(2),260–271.
Kim,J.,&Ross,C.E.(2009).Neighborhood-specificandgeneral
socialsupport:Whichbufferstheeffectofneighborhood
disorderondepression? JournalofCommunityPsychology,
37(6),725–736.
Kirk,D.S.,&Matsuda,M.(2011).Legalcynicism,collective
efficacy,andtheecologyofarrest. Criminology,49(2),443–472.
Kirst,M.,Lazgare,L.P.,Zhang,Y.J.,&O’Campo,P.(2015).The
effectsofsocialcapitalandneighborhoodcharacteristicson
intimatepartnerviolence:Aconsiderationofsocialresources
andrisks. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,55,
314–325.
Kornhauser,R.R.(1978). Socialsourcesofdelinquency:An
appraisalofanalyticmodels.Chicago,IL:UniversityofChicago
Press.
Latkin,C.A.,&Curry,A.D.(2003).Stressfulneighborhoodsand
depression:Aprospectivestudyoftheimpactofneighborhood
disorder. JournalofHealthandSocialBehavior,44(1),34–44.
Mair,C.,DiezRoux,A.V.,&Galea,S.(2008).Areneighbourhood
characteristicsassociatedwithdepressivesymptoms?Areview
ofevidence. JournalofEpidemiologyandCommunityHealth,
62(11),940–946.
Mair,C.,DiezRoux,A.V.,&Morenoff,J.D.(2010).Neighborhood
stressorsandsocialsupportaspredictorsofdepressivesymp-
tomsintheChicagoCommunityAdultHealthStudy. Health&
Place,16(5),811–819.
Miles-Doan,R.(1998).Violencebetweenspousesandintimates:Does
neighborhoodcontextmatter? SocialForces,77(2),623–645.
Moe,A.M.(2007).Silencedvoicesandstructuredsurvival:Battered
women’shelpseeking. ViolenceAgainstWomen,13(7),676–699.
Molnar,B.E.,Cerda,M.,Roberts,A.L.,&Buka,S.L.(2008).
Effectsofneighborhoodresourcesonaggressiveanddelinquent
behaviorsamongurbanyouths. AmericanJournalofPublic
Health,98(6),1086–1093.
Molnar,B.E.,Miller,M.J.,Azrael,D.,&Buka,S.L.(2004).
Neighborhoodpredictorsofconcealedfirearmcarryingamong
childrenandadolescents:Resultsfromtheprojectonhuman
developmentinChicagoneighborhoods. ArchivesofPediatrics
andAdolescentMedicine,158(7),657–664.
Morenoff,J.D.,Sampson,R.J.,&Raudenbush,S.W.(2001).
Neighborhoodinequality,collectiveefficacy,andthespatial
dynamicsofurbanviolence. Criminology,39(3),517–559.
O’Campo,P.,Burke,J.,Peak,G.L.,McDonnell,K.A.,&Gielen,A.
C.(2005).Uncoveringneighbourhoodinfluenceonintimate
partnerviolenceusingconceptmapping. JournalofEpidemiol-
ogyandCommunityHealth,59(7),603–608.
Osgood,D.W.,McMorris,B.J.,&Potenza,M.T.(2002).Analyzing
multiple-itemmeasuresofcrimeanddevianceI:Itemresponse
theoryscaling. JournalofQuantitativeCriminology,18,
267–296.
Pinchevsky,G.M.,&Wright,E.M.(2012).Theimpactof
neighborhoodsonintimatepartnerviolenceandvictimization.
Trauma,Violence,&Abuse,12(2),112–132.
Raudenbush,S.W.,Johnson,C.,&Sampson,R.J.(2003).A
multivariate,multilevelRaschmodelwithapplicationtoself-
reportedcriminalbehavior. SociologicalMethodology,33(1),
169–211.
Rios,R.,Aiken,L.S.,&Zautra,A.J.(2012).Neighborhoodcontexts
andthemediatingroleofneighborhoodsocialcohesionon
healthandpsychologicaldistressamongHispanicandnon-
Hispanicresidents. AnnalsofBehavioralMedicine,43(1),
50–61.doi:10.1007/s12160-011-9306-9.
Ross,C.E.(2000).Neighborhooddisadvantageandadultdepression.
JournalofHealthandSocialBehavior,41(2),177–187.
Ross,C.E.,&Jang,S.J.(2000).Neighborhooddisorder,fear,and
mistrust:Thebufferingroleofsocialtieswithneighbors.
AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,28(4),401–420.
Ross,C.E.,&Mirowsky,J.(2009).Neighborhooddisorder,
subjectivealienation,anddistress. JournalofHealthandSocial
Behavior,50(1),49–64.doi:10.1177/002214650905000104.
Ross,C.E.,Mirowsky,J.,&Pribesh,S.(2001).Powerlessandthe
amplificationofthreat:Neighborhooddisadvantage,disorder,
andmistrust. AmericanSociologicalReview,66(4),568–591.
Ross,C.E.,Reynolds,J.R.,&Geis,K.J.(2000).Thecontingent
meaningofneighborhoodstabilityforresidents’psychological
well-being. AmericanSociologicalReview,65(4),581–597.
Sampson,R.J.(2003).Theneighborhoodcontextofwellbeing.
PerspectivesinBiologyandMedicine,46(3),S53–S73.
Sampson,R.J.(2013). GreatAmericancity:Chicagoandthe
enduringneighborhoodeffect.Chicago,IL:Universityof
ChicagoPress.
Sampson,R.J.,Morenoff,J.D.,&Raudenbush,S.W.(2005).Social
anatomyofracialandethnicdisparatiesinviolence. American
JournalofPublicHealth,95(2),224–232.
AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 355
123Sampson,R.J.,Raudenbush,S.W.,&Earls,F.J.(1997).
Neighborhoodsandviolentcrime:Amultilevelstudyof
collectiveefficacy. Science,277(5328),918–924.
Shaw,C.R.,&McKay,H.D.(1942). Juveniledelinquencyandurban
areas:Astudyofratesofdelinquentsinrelationtodifferential
characteristicsoflocalcommunitiesinAmericancities.Chicago,
IL:UniversityofChicagoPress.
Stafford,M.,McMunn,A.,&DeVogli,R.(2011).Neighbourhood
socialenvironmentanddepressivesymptomsinmid-lifeand
beyond. Ageing&Society,31(6),893–910.
Stith,S.M.,Smith,D.B.,Penn,C.E.,Ward,D.B.,&Tritt,D.
(2004).Intimatepartnerphysicalabuseperpetrationandvictim-
izationriskfactors:Ameta-analyticreview. Aggressionand
ViolentBehavior,10(1),65–98.
Straus,M.A.(1979).Measuringintrafamilyconflictandviolence:
Theconflicttactics(CT)scales. JournalofMarriageand
Family,41(1),75–88.
Straus,M.A.,Gelles,R.J.,&Steinmetz,S.K.(2006). Behindclosed
doors:ViolenceintheAmericanfamily.NewBrunswick,NJ:
TransactionPublishers.
Taylor,S.E.,&Repetti,R.L.(1997).Healthpsychology:Whatisan
unhealthyenvironmentandhowdoesitgetundertheskin?
AnnualReviewofPsychology,48,411–447.
VanWyk,J.A.,Benson,M.L.,Fox,G.L.,&DeMaris,A.(2003).
Detanglingindividual-,partner-,andcommunity-levelcorrelates
ofpartnerviolence. Crime&Delinquency,49(3),412–438.
Vega,W.A.,Ang,A.,Rodriguez,M.A.,&Finch,B.K.(2011).
NeighborhoodprotectiveeffectsondepressioninLatinos.
AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,47(1–2),114–126.
Warner,B.D.(2014).Neighborhoodfactorsrelatedtothelikelihood
ofsuccessfulinformalsocialcontrolefforts. JournalofCriminal
Justice,42,421–430.
Wellman,B.,&Wortley,S.(1990).Differentstrokesfromdifferent
folks:Communitytiesandsocialsupport. AmericanJournalof
Sociology,96(3),558–588.
Wilson,J.Q.,&Kelling,G.L.(1982).Thepoliceandneighborhood
safety:Brokenwindows. AtlanticMonthly,127,29–38.
Wright,E.M.(2012).Therelationshipbetweensocialsupportand
intimatepartnerviolenceinneighborhoodcontext. Crime&
Delinquency.doi:10.1177/0011128712466890.
Wright,E.M.,&Benson,M.L.(2010).Immigrationandintimate
partnerviolence:Exploringtheimmigrantparadox. Social
Problems,57(3),480–503.
Wright,E.M.,&Benson,M.L.(2011).Clarifyingtheeffectsof
neighborhooddisadvantageandcollectiveefficacyonviolence
‘‘behindcloseddoors’’. JusticeQuarterly,28(5),775–798.
Xue,Y.,Leventhal,T.,Brooks-Gunn,J.,&Earls,F.J.(2005).
Neighborhoodresidenceandmentalhealthproblemsof5-to
11-yearolds. ArchivesofGeneralPsychiatry,62(5),554–563.
Zimmerman,G.M.,&Messner,S.F.(2010).Neighborhoodcontext
andthegendergapinadolescentviolentcrime. American
SociologicalReview,75(6),958–980.
Zimmerman,G.M.,&Messner,S.F.(2011).Neighborhoodcontext
andnonlinearpeereffectsonadolescentviolentcrime. Crimi-
nology,49(3),873–903.
356 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356
123Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.