ORIGINALARTICLE IntimatePartnerViolenceandSubsequentDepression: ExaminingtheRolesofNeighborhoodSupportiveMechanisms EmilyM.Wright 1 • GillianM.Pinchevsky2 • MichaelL.Benson3 • DanaL.Radatz4 Publishedonline:21September2015  SocietyforCommunityResearchandAction2015 Abstract Thisstudyexaminesthedirecteffectsof neighborhoodsupportivemechanisms(e.g.,collective efficacy,socialcohesion,socialnetworks)ondepressive symptomsamongfemalesaswellastheirmoderating effectsontheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepressive symptoms.Amultilevel,multivariateRaschmodelwas usedwithdatafromtheProjectonHumanDevelopmentin ChicagoNeighborhoodstoassesstheexistenceofIPVand latersusceptibilityofdepressivesymptomsamong2959 adultfemalesin80neighborhoods.Resultsindicatethat neighborhoodcollectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,social interactions,andthenumberoffriendsandfamilyinthe neighborhoodreducethelikelihoodthatfemalesexperi- encedepressivesymptoms.However,livinginareaswith highproportionsoffriendsandrelativesexacerbatesthe impactofIPVonfemales’subsequentdepressivesymp- toms.Thefindingsindicatethatneighborhoodsupportive mechanismsimpactinterpersonaloutcomesinbothdirect andmoderatingways,althoughdirecteffectsweremore pronouncedfordepressionthanmoderatingeffects.Future researchshouldcontinuetoexaminethepositiveand potentiallymitigatinginfluencesofneighborhoodsinorder tobetterunderstandforwhomandunderwhichcircum- stancesviolentrelationshipsandmentalhealthareinflu- encedbycontextualfactors. Keywords Intimatepartnerviolence  Depression  Neighborhoods  Protectivefactors  Collectiveefficacy  Socialties Introduction Intimatepartnerviolence(IPV)anddepressionaremajor publichealthconcernsthatarebothimpactedbyneigh- borhoodcontext(Bensonetal. 2003;Kim 2008;Mairetal. 2008;Miles-Doan 1998;PinchevskyandWright 2012; Ross 2000).Themajorityofextantresearch,however,has tendedtofocusonthedirectandnegativeimpactof neighborhoodsontheseoutcomes,whilelittleresearchhas exploredthepotentialprotectiveeffectsof positive neigh- borhoodconditions.Forinstance,thereissomeresearch whichsuggestssupportivemechanismssuchassocial cohesion,socialties,orcollectiveefficacymayprotect individualsfromIPVordepressionandothermentalhealth issues(AneshenselandSucoff 1996;Emeryetal. 2011; GeisandRoss 1998;Kirstetal. 2015;Pinchevskyand Wright 2012;RossandJang 2000;Rossetal. 2000;Wright andBenson 2010, 2011),butmostofthesestudiesassess onlyneighborhooddirecteffectsandtreattheseoutcomes & EmilyM.Wright [email protected] GillianM.Pinchevsky [email protected] MichaelL.Benson [email protected] DanaL.Radatz [email protected] 1 SchoolofCriminologyandCriminalJustice,University ofNebraska,6001DodgeStreet,218CPACS,Omaha, NE68182-0149,USA 2 DepartmentofCriminalJustice,UniversityofNevada, LasVegas,4505SouthMarylandParkway, Box455009,LasVegas,NV89154,USA 3 SchoolofCriminalJustice,UniversityofCincinnati, 665HDyerHall,Cincinnati,OH45221,USA 4 DepartmentofCriminology&CriminalJustice,Niagara University,P.O.Box1941,NewYork,NY14109-1941,USA 123 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 DOI10.1007/s10464-015-9753-8separately.Mostnotablyforthecurrentstudy,verylittle researchhasinvestigatedthepotentialforneighborhood supportivefeaturestomoderateimportantindividual-level relationships,suchastheimpactofIPVonsubsequent depression. Thus,whileweknowthatneighborhoodscandetri- mentallyimpactIPVanddepression(LatkinandCurry 2003;Mairetal. 2008;PinchevskyandWright 2012),we knowlessaboutthepositiveimpactthatneighborhood supportivefeaturesmighthaveontheseproblems.Addi- tionally,wedonotknowwhether,orhow,neighborhood supportivemechanismsmoderatetheimpactofIPVon subsequentdepression.Weattempttoaddtothesparse researchinthisareabyexaminingtherelationshipbetween intimatepartnerviolenceandsubsequentdepressionin neighborhoodcontext.Weusedatafrom2959females livingin80neighborhoodsinChicagotoinvestigatethe directeffectsofneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms, suchascollectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,andinterac- tionswithothers,onfemales’depressivesymptoms,as wellastoexplorewhetherneighborhoodcontextalleviates thelong-termimpactofexperiencingIPVonlater depressionamongtheseadults. IntimatePartnerViolenceVictimization andDepression Scholarshavesuggestedthatvictimsofpartnerviolence maybeparticularlysusceptibletoexperiencingmental healthproblemsintheaftermathofabuseinpartbecause theirvictimization(andtheassociatedtrauma)isinflicted bypeoplewhomthevictimtrusts,loves,orconsiderstobe ‘‘safe’’(DeMarisandKaukinen 2005).Alongwithpost- traumaticstressdisorder,depressionisamongthemost prevalentmentalhealthconsequencesofIPV(Caldwell etal. 2012;Campbell 2002),withover45%ofvictims experiencingit(Golding 1999).Althoughdepression amongvictimsmaydeclineifandwhenthevictimization does,long-termeffectsdolinger,butmoreresearchinthis areaisneeded,especiallyamongadultpopulations (Bonomietal. 2006;Campbell 2002;Cokeretal. 2002a; Fletcher 2010;Johnsonetal. 2014). Violencefrompartnerscanbelinkedtodepressionvia mechanismssuchasstress,powerlessness,hopelessness, isolation,lowself-esteem,andphysicalpain(Campbell etal. 1996;Goodmanetal. 2009).Asasourceofstress, abusecancontributetodepressionbydisruptingdaily routines,increasingotherstressfuleventsinone’slife, loweringthevictim’sfeelingsofsecurityandsenseofself- esteem,orincreasingtheirfeelingsofpowerlessnessto controlthesituation(Campbell 2002;Campbelletal. 1996; Goodmanetal. 2009).Forinstance,becauseofthephysical painandinjurythatisoftenaconsequenceofIPV,victims maydevelopchronicpainorsufferconsequencesofthe abuse(e.g.,episodesoffaintingcausedbyextensivehead trauma;sexuallytransmitteddiseases,etc.,seeCampbell 2002),whichmayleadtodepressionovertime.Inaddition, socialisolationfromothers,whichisoftenpurposefully achievedbyabusersinviolentrelationships(e.g.,Campbell etal. 1996),canalsoleadtodepressionbecauseisolated victimsmayfeelthattheyhavenoonetoturntoforhelpor support.WhiletherearemanyavenuesbywhichIPVmay leadtodepressionandothernegativementalhealthdis- positions,fewstudieshaveexaminedsuchrelationships withlongitudinaldataamongadults,andmoreassessment isneededinthisarea(Campbell 2002;Fletcher 2010). Moreover,researchhasneglectedtoexaminethisrela- tionshipfromacontextualperspective. TheImportanceofNeighborhoodContext Neighborhoodcontextisrelatedtodepressionandother problematicmentalhealthoutcomesamongbothadults (e.g.,Kim 2008;Mairetal. 2008)andchildren(e.g.,Xue etal. 2005).Theliteratureregardingtheneighborhood predictorsofdepressiontendstocenteronsocioeconomic, poverty-related,orsocialdisordervariables,andgenerally demonstratesthatlowersocioeconomicstatus(SES)and moreneighborhooddisorderdetrimentallyimpactdepres- sionandothernegativementalhealthoutcomes.Mairetal. (2008)foundthat,of45studiesonneighborhoodsand depression,37demonstratedsignificantneighborhood effects,evenaftercontrollingforindividual-levelfactors suchasage,race,gender,andindicatorsofsocioeconomic status;inanotherreview,Kim(2008)foundthat11of22 studiesexaminingneighborhoodsocioeconomicstatus showedthatneighborhoodconditionshadsignificant effectsondepression.Mechanismsdirectlylinkingneigh- borhoodfactorstomentalhealthordepressionoften revolvearoundincreasedstresslevels,limitedaccesstoor scantresources,disorder,violence,inadequatehousing, andalackofpublicaccessorgreenspaces,suchasbike lanesandcleanstreetsorsidewalks(e.g.,Kim 2008;Mair etal. 2008;TaylorandRepetti 1997).Accumulatingevi- dencealsosuggeststhatperceiveddisorderand/ordan- gerousnessoftheneighborhoodincreasesdepression, perhapsinpartduetoincreasedfearormistrustamong neighbors(e.g.,RossandJang 2000;RossandMirowsky 2009),socialisolation(e.g.,GeisandRoss 1998;Rossand Mirowsky 2009),feelingsofpowerlessness(e.g.,Anesh- enselandSucoff 1996;GeisandRoss 1998),orfearand anxietyamongstneighbors(e.g.,Hilletal. 2005).Inshort, unkempt,rundown,disorderly,anddisorganizedneigh- borhoodenvironmentscanbedemoralizing,distressing, threatening,andisolatingtoresidentsandcansendthe AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 343 123messagethatnoonecaresabouttheirwellbeingorthatof theneighborhood(RossandMirowsky 2009;Sampson 2013;WilsonandKelling 1982).Allofthesefactorsmight, inturn,contributetodepression. Despitethedeleteriouseffectsofneighborhoodson negativeoutcomes,itisalsopossiblethatneighborhoods may mitigate theeffectsofstressors—includingviolence suchasIPV—byorganizingcommunityresidentstogether andfosteringsocialcohesionand/orinterconnectedness amongthem.Thereissomeevidencesupportingneigh- borhoodprotectiveeffectsagainstmentalhealthoutcomes, butthisbodyofresearchprimarilyfocusesonneighbor- hoodcohesionandcollectiveefficacy.Intheirreview,Mair etal.(2008)found11studiesthatreportedthatpositive interactionsbetweenneighbors—includingincreasedsocial connectionsandsupportfromresidents,increasedcom- municationbetweenresidents,andreducedsocialisola- tion—servedasprotectivefactorsagainstdepression.Ross andcolleagues(KimandRoss 2009;RossandJang 2000) havedemonstratedthatneighborhoodsocialtiesandsocial supportreducedepressionandotherpsychologicaldistress, includingperceivedpowerlessness,fear,andmistrust(see alsoKim 2010).Neighborhoodsocialcohesionhasalso beenlinkedtoreduceddepression(Mairetal. 2010);Ahern andGalea(2011)suggestthatsocialcohesionwithina neighborhoodmayhelpresidentsexertsocialcontrol, whichcanprovidemoresupportbetweenneighbors,reduce thenumberofstressorsthatresidentsperceivetoexist withintheneighborhood,andbufferormitigatethestres- sorswhichdooccur.Further,theynotethatcohesion betweenresidentsmayfostercommunication—whichcan keepresidentsknowledgeableabouttheircommunity— improvethelocalservicesandresourcesthatareavailable, andincreaseapersonalsenseofcontrol,whichinturn,can alleviatedepressivesymptoms(Staffordetal. 2011).Areas whereresidentsknoweachother,communicate,and interactare,infact,perceivedtofosterbettermentalhealth amongresidents:forinstance,Torontoresidentsperceived conceptssuchas‘‘interactionbetweenneighbors,’’ ‘‘knowingneighbors,’’‘‘socialcohesion,’’and‘‘communi- cationbetweenresidents’’tobeimportantpiecesof neighborhoodsupportwhichwerethoughtberelatedto positivementalhealth(Burkeetal. 2009).Indeed,Samp- son(2013)suggeststhatresidentsinneighborhoodsofhigh collectiveefficacy(i.e.,wheretherearesharedexpecta- tionsaboutnormsandbehavior)arebetterabletoachieve commongoalsandaremorelikelytoengagein(and benefitfrom)sociallyaltruisticbehaviors. 1 Suchbenefits areunsurprisinglyassociatedwithbettermentalhealth— collectiveefficacy,forinstance,hasbeenfoundtoreduce depressionamonglong-termLatinoimmigrantsinthe UnitedStates(Vegaetal. 2011)aswellasamongolder adults(AhernandGalea 2011),andisassociatedwithother sociallyaltruisticbehaviorssuchasbystanderintervention (Edwardsetal. 2014). Itisnecessarytoaccountforneighborhooddisadvantage whenexaminingtheimpactofneighborhoodprotective factors,however,becausedisadvantagecanimpedethe qualityandquantityofthehelpingmechanismswhichare available(Goodmanetal. 2009;Sampson 2003).Inother words,themechanismsdescribedabovethatmightmiti- gatedepressionamongresidentsmaybelesslikelytobe presentindisadvantagedneighborhoods.Forinstance, scholarshavesuggestedthattheformationandextentof socialtiesbetweenresidentsmaybelowerindisadvan- tagedareas(seeKornhauser 1978;ShawandMcKay 1942) potentiallybecauseoftheirfear,mistrust,andperceptions ofdangerousness(RossandMirowsky 2009;Rossetal. 2001).Havingfeworweaksocialtiesbetweenneighbors mightreducehelp-seekingbehaviorsamongvictimsof partnerviolence. Additionally,someoftheliteratureonneighborhood protectiveeffectsondepressionandmentalhealthout- comeshasbeenlimitedinotherways.Forinstance,data thatisreportedbyparticipantsbutaggregatedtothe neighborhoodlevel(e.g.,Echeverriaetal. 2008;Mairetal. 2010;Riosetal. 2012)risksconflatingoutcomeswith predictorvariables(i.e.,neighborhoodmeasures).While theresearchbasehasexaminedbothneighborhood-level tiesandsupports,therehavealsobeenstudieswhichfocus ontiesandsupportivemechanismsattheindividual-level onlyoruseaggregatedmeasures(e.g.,Echeverriaetal. 2008;GeisandRoss 1998;Riosetal. 2012;RossandJang 2000).Further,manystudieshaveutilizedcross-sectional designs(Mairetal. 2008),whichcannotascertainwhether neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsprecedeincreased depressionorwhetherpeopleseekoutsupporttohelpwith theirdepression.Mostimportantlytothecurrentstudy, researchhasneglectedtoexaminetheeffectsofneigh- borhoodsupportivemechanismsontheIPV—depression relationshipspecifically. NeighborhoodSupportsMayMitigatetheEffect ofPartnerViolenceonLaterDepression WeexaminewhethertherelationshipbetweenIPVand subsequentdepressiondependsinpartupontheneighbor- hoodinwhichitoccurs.Basedonpreviousresearchand theory,wemightexpectthatneighborhoodsupportive mechanisms,suchasstrongsocialcohesion,collective efficacy,orsocialnetworksbetweenresidentswillboth 1 However,thebeneficialeffectsofcollectiveefficacyonvarious outcomesmaybelimitedorhinderedwhennormssuchaslegal cynicismaretakenintoaccount(Emeryetal. 2011;KirkandMatsuda 2011). 344 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123directlydecreasedepressionandalleviatetheeffectsofIPV ondepression,despitethelevelofdisadvantagethatexists withinaneighborhood.Althoughwedonotdirectlytest thesemechanisms,wesuspectthattheseneighborhood protectivefactorswillincreasethesupportthatisavailable fromotherstothevictim,leadingtolowerdepressionandan attenuatedimpactofIPVonthisoutcome.Indeed,Kirstetal. (2015)foundthatTorontoresidentswithhighperceived socialsupportandlargersocialnetworkstodrawuponwere lesslikelytoexperienceIPV.Wright(2012)suggestedthat victimsofpartnerviolencemayrelyonsocialsupportfrom othersforemotionalandfinancialhelpwhenabuseoccurs, andthatthesesupportsystemsmayaidethevictiminleaving theabusiverelationshiptemporarilyorpermanently.In addition,supportivenetworkswithothersmayprovidevic- timswithadviceregardingsafeplacestogoortheservices thatareavailabletotheminordertoescapetheviolence (HadeedandEl-Bassel 2006;Moe 2007;Wright 2012).In short,neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsmayprovide optionsforvictimstorelyonwheninneed.Second,we believethatcommunicationwithresidentsintheneighbor- hoodmayincreasethelikelihoodthatvictimstellothers abouttheirvictimization(Browning 2002),andthisopen communicationmayhelpvictimscope(Cokeretal. 2002b) aswellasincreasethelikelihoodthatotherswilllearnabout thevictimizationandpotentiallyintervene(Wright 2012; WrightandBenson 2011).Havingmoreavenuesavailableto victimsforcopingwiththeviolenceorseekinghelpto alleviatetheabusemayreducethestresscausedbythe victimization.Finally,supportiveneighborhoodsmay reduceresidents’feelingsofisolationand/ormistrust,which inturnmayreducevictims’feelingsofpowerlessness. Weexaminethesepossibilitiesinthecurrentstudyand focusonthreeresearchquestions:(1)whatarethelong-term effectsofexperiencingintimatepartnerviolenceonthe likelihoodthatfemaleslaterreportdepressivesymptoms, whilecontrollingforotherimportantindividualandneigh- borhoodcovariates?(2)whatarethedirecteffectsof neighborhoodsupports(e.g.,collectiveefficacy,social cohesion,andsocialnetworkswithothers)onfemales’ depressivesymptoms,accountingforindividualcovariates andneighborhooddisadvantage?and(3)dotheseneigh- borhoodsupportivemechanismsalleviatetheimpactofIPV ondepressivesymptoms3yearslater,aftercontrollingfor relevantindividualandneighborhoodcovariates? Methods Data DataforthisstudyweredrawnfromtheProjecton HumanDevelopmentinChicagoNeighborhoods (PHDCN;Earlsetal. 2002),amulti-component,multi- wavestudywhichallowsfortheexaminationofhow neighborhoodcontextmayimpactindividual-levelout- comes.Thisstudyreliesondatafromthreeseparate componentsofthePHDCN:theLongitudinalCohort Study(LCS),theCommunitySurvey,andthe1990U.S. Census.PHDCNresearchersdividedChicago’s847cen- sustractsinto343geographicallycontinuousneighbor- hoodclusters(NCs).These343NCswerethenstratified bysevencategoriesofracial/ethnicdiversityandthree levelsofsocio-economicstatus,and80NCswerethen selectedviaprobabilitysampling.Withinthese80NCs, multiplecohortsofeligibleyouthandtheirprimary caregivers(93%ofwhomwerefemale)wereselectedfor inclusionintheLCS.Thecurrentstudyutilizesdata collectedfromfemalecaregivers(hereafterreferredtoas respondents)duringthefirst(1994–1997)andsecond (1997–2000)wavesoftheLCS. Neighborhoodmeasuresrelatedtocollectiveefficacy, cohesion,andsocialinteractionsortiesweretakenfrom thePHDCNCommunitySurveywhilemeasuresfor neighborhooddisadvantagewereabstractedfromthe1990 UnitedStatesCensus. 2 TheCommunitySurvey(con- ductedin1994–1995)sampledresidentsfromtheoriginal 343NCsandaskedabouttheirperceptionsofneighbor- hoodsocialprocesses,includingsocialnetworksand interactionsbetweenresidents,organizationalgroups, values,andsoforth.Usingathree-stagesamplingdesign, cityblocksweresampledwithineachNC,dwellingunits weresampledwithinblocks,andoneadultresidentwas sampledwithineachdwellingunit.Thus,participantsof theCommunitySurveywerelargelyindependentofthose drawnfortheLCS. 3 Thecurrentstudyincludes2959 femalerespondentslivingin80neighborhoodclusters (hereafterreferredtoasneighborhoods)whoweremar- ried,cohabitating,orinadatingrelationshipatwaveone andansweredquestionsrelatedtodepressivesymptomsat wavetwo. 4 2 StaffattheInter-universityConsortiumforPoliticalandSocial ResearchcalculatedNC-linkedU.S.Censusmeasuresinorderto ensuretheconfidentialityofthesubjectsofthePHDCN. 3 AlthoughtheCommunitySurveycollectedinformationfromall 343NCsaboutneighborhoodconditionsviainterviewswiththese residents,thisstudyfocusesonlyonthose80NCsinwhichthe participantsoftheLCSwerenested. 4 Toarriveatthe2959,atotalof292casesweredeleteddueto missingdata.Theonlysignificantdifferencebetweenouranalysis sampleandtheeligiblesampleoffemalecaregiversinarelationship wasthatouranalysissamplehadslightlyfewerHispanicwomen (p\.05).Therewerenosignificantdifferencesonthemain independentvariableofinterestoranyothercontrolvariables. AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 345 123Measures DepressionSymptoms Thirteendepressionmeasureswereadaptedfromtheshort formoftheCompositeInternationalDiagnosticInterview (UM-CIDI;Kessleretal. 1998)andwereself-reportedby thefemalerespondentsatwavetwo.Respondentswerefirst askedquestionsregardingwhethertheyhadfeltsad,blue, ordepressedfortwoormoreweeksinarowduringthe pastyear.Ifrespondentsansweredaffirmatively,theywere askedfollow-upquestionsregardingtheperiodoftime theyexperienceddepressivesymptomology(i.e.,during the2-weekperiodwhentheyfeltsadorblue,didtheylose interestinmostthings;feeltiredorhavelowenergy; experienceaweightchangeof10ormorepounds;have troublefallingasleeporconcentrating;experiencefeelings ofworthlessness;thinkaboutdeath).Respondentswere alsoaskedthesamefollow-upquestionsregardingany 2-week(ormore)periodoftimeinthepastyearwhenthey lostinterestinmostthingslikehobbies,work,oractivities thattheyusuallyenjoyed.This13-itemscalehadarelia- bilityof.902. Topredicttheoddsofexperiencingdepressivesymp- toms,weusedamultivariate,multilevelRaschmodel (Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thethree-levelmodelnests depressionitemresponseswithinpersonswithinneigh- borhoods.Thelevel-1model(itemswithinpersons)pro- ducesalatentvariablethatrepresentseachperson’s susceptibilityfor depressionsymptoms (i.e.,theirlikeli- hoodofexperiencingdepressivesymptoms).Thiscontin- uousvariableisassumedtobenormallydistributedona logitmetricandistheoutcomeforthelevel-two(respon- dent-level)andlevel-three(neighborhoodlevel)models (Osgoodetal. 2002). IntimatePartnerViolence SevereIPV wasassessedusingtheConflictTacticsScale forParentsandSpouses(Straus 1979)andreflectsthe prevalenceofseveremale-partner-perpetratedaggression againstthefemalerespondentinthepastyear.Respondents reportedwhethertheirmalepartnerusedanyofsixforms ofsevereaggressionorviolenceagainsttheminthepast year:hitwithafist,hitwithsomething,beatup,choked, threatenedwithaknifeoragun,orusedaknifeoragun (0 = no;1 = yes).Thismeasurereflectspartnervictim- izationatwaveone. NeighborhoodVariables Drawingfrompriorresearch(Cerdaetal. 2008;Molnar etal. 2004, 2008), concentrateddisadvantage was operationalizedasaprincipalcomponentsfactoranalysis includingthepercentageofresidentsinaneighborhood whowerelivingbelowthepovertyline,receivingpublic assistance,andunemployed(alpha = .805).Highervalues reflectgreatereconomicdisadvantage.Becauseofthe possibleconfoundingeffectsofdisadvantageonneigh- borhoodsocialsupportmechanisms,concentrateddisad- vantageislargelyincludedasacontrolmeasure. 5 Weusetwomeasuresthatreflectthedegreeoftrustand supportbetweenneighborsinthecommunity.Following Sampsonetal.(1997),neighborhood collectiveefficacy wasbasedon10itemsfromadultsparticipatinginthe CommunitySurveyandreflectsthedegreeofsocial cohesionandinformalsocialcontrolbetweenneighbors. Residentswereaskedhowstronglytheyagreed(onafive- pointLikertscalerangingfrom‘‘stronglydisagree’’to ‘‘stronglyagree’’)that:peoplearoundherearewillingto helptheirneighbors;thisisaclose-knitneighborhood; peopleinthisneighborhoodcanbetrusted;peopleinthis neighborhoodgenerallydon’tgetalongwitheachother (reversecoded);andpeopleinthisneighborhooddonot sharethesamevalues(reversecoded).Residentswerealso askedfiveitemsregardingthelikelihood(assessedona five-pointLikertscalefrom‘‘veryunlikely’’to‘‘very likely’’)thattheirneighborswouldinterveneif:children wereskippingschoolandhangingoutonastreetcorner; childrenwerespraypaintinggraffitionalocalbuilding; childrenwereshowingdisrespecttoanadult;afightbroke outinfrontoftheirhouse;andthefirestationclosestto theirhomewasthreatenedwithbudgetcuts.Following Sampsonetal.(1997)andothers(Browningetal. 2004; Morenoffetal. 2001),theseitemswerecombinedintoa singlemeasureofcollectiveefficacyusingathree-level itemresponsemodel. 6 Thelevel-onemodeladjustedthe within-personcollectiveefficacyscoresbyitemdifficulty, missingdata,andmeasurementerror.Thelevel-twomodel estimatedneighborhoodcollectiveefficacyscoresadjusting forthesocialcompositionofeachneighborhood.Inpar- ticular,potentialbiasesinperceptionsofeachconstruct 5 Wecontrolforneighborhooddisadvantageinmultilevelanalyses because,relativetootherstructuralconditionssuchasresidential mobilityorethnicheterogeneity,disadvantagehasbeenfoundtobe themostconsistentinfluenceonbothIPVanddepression(Kim 2008; Mairetal. 2008;PinchevskyandWright 2012). 6 Itemresponsemodelingtechniquesavoidthelossofdatafrom missingresponsestoasetofquestionsorindicators(Osgoodetal. 2002),takeitemdifficultyintoaccount(i.e.,thatsomeindicatorsof neighborhoodconstructsmaybemoredifficultandlessprevalentthan others),andallowsimultaneousestimationoftheimpactofindivid- ual-levelinfluences(e.g.,age,gender)onperceptionsofthese constructs(Sampsonetal. 2005).Theitemresponsemodelsusedin thisstudyultimatelyprovidetheneighborhood-levelofcollective efficacy(or,socialcohesion,orsocialinteraction)aftertheseissues havebeenaccountedfor. 346 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123resultingfromcharacteristicsrelatedtogender,marital status,homeownership,ethnicityandrace,residential mobility,yearsintheneighborhood,age,andsocioeco- nomicstatuswerecontrolledatlevel-two.Finally,the level-threemodelallowedeachneighborhoodcluster’s meancollectiveefficacyscoretovaryrandomlyarounda grandmean.TheempiricalBayesresidualfromthelevel- threemodelconstitutestheneighborhoodlevelofcollec- tiveefficacyaftercontrollingforitemdifficultyand neighborhoodsocialcompositionandwasthereforeusedas the‘true’neighborhoodscoreoncollectiveefficacy.The internalconsistencyofthisscaleattheneighborhoodlevel was.847. Ithasbeensuggestedthatcohesionbetweenresidents mayuniquelyimpacttheirmentalwell-being(Stafford etal. 2011),andKirstetal.’s(2015)findingssuggestthere areuniquecontributionsofdifferentformsofneighbor- hoodsocialcapital(e.g.,collectiveefficacy,perceived socialsupport,individualnetworksize)withregardtoIPV. Therefore,wewishedtoseparateouttheeffectsofsocial cohesionfromsocialcontrol,asbotharecapturedinthe overallcollectiveefficacymeasuredescribedabove. 7 We createdaseparatemeasureof socialcohesion,apartfrom thecollectiveefficacymeasure,andmodeledthemsepa- ratelyintheanalyses.Socialcohesionwasbasedonfive itemsaskedoftheCommunitySurveyparticipants;resi- dentswereaskedhowstronglytheyagreed(from‘‘strongly disagree’’to‘‘stronglyagree’’)that:peoplearoundhereare willingtohelptheirneighbors;thisisaclose-knitneigh- borhood;peopleinthisneighborhoodcanbetrusted; peopleinthisneighborhoodgenerallydon’tgetalongwith eachother(reversecoded);andpeopleinthisneighbor- hooddonotsharethesamevalues(reversecoded).Inde- pendentlyfromcollectiveefficacy,socialcohesionwas alsooperationalizedusingathree-levelitemresponse model.Asdescribedabove,theempiricalBayesresidual fromthethree-levelmodelconstitutestheneighborhood scoreonsocialcohesionaftercontrollingforsocialcohe- sionitem-difficultyandneighborhoodsocialcomposition (e.g.,respondentcharacteristicssuchasage,race,etc.).The neighborhood-levelinternalconsistencyofthisscalewas .826. Oneitemtappingthelevelofsocialinteractionbetween neighborswasalsoincludedinthisstudy.Adultspartici- patingintheCommunitySurveywereaskedfourquestions abouttheir socialinteractions withotherneighborhood residents.Respondentswereaskedhowoften(onafour- pointLikertscale,from‘‘never’’to‘‘often’’)theyand peopleintheneighborhood:dofavorsforeachother;ask eachotheradviceaboutpersonalthings;haveget-togethers whereotherpeopleintheneighborhoodareinvited;and visitineachother’shomesoronthestreet.Similartothe otherneighborhoodsocialprocessesvariables,athree-level itemresponsemodelwasusedtocreatethesocialinter- actionsscale(seeBrowningetal. 2004).Theinternal consistencyofthisscaleattheneighborhoodlevelwas .734. Finally,threemeasureswereincludedtoassessthe existenceoffriendandfamilysocialtiesintheneighbor- hood.ThesemeasureswerealsotakenfromtheCommu- nitySurvey. Anyfriends and anyfamily werecomputed basedonresidents’answerstowhethertheyhadany friendsandanyrelativesorin-laws,respectively,livingin theirneighborhood(excludingthosefriendsorfamily memberswithwhomtheylived).Theseresponseswere thenaggregatedtotheneighborhood-level.Finally,the averagenumberoffriendsandfamily representsthe numberoffriendsandfamily/in-lawslivingintheneigh- borhood(eachonascaleof0–4,indicatingnoneto10or morefriendsorfamily/in-laws)averaged,andaggregated totheneighborhood-level. ControlVariables Additionalrespondent-levelfactorsdemonstratedinprior researchtobeassociatedwiththeoddsofdepressionand intimatepartnerviolencewerealsoincludedinanalyses (Stithetal. 2004).Suchvariablesincludedemographic predictors(age,race/ethnicity,married[versusdatingor cohabitingwithapartner],householdsalary,andemploy- mentstatus),opportunityvariables(isolation),andprior lifehistories(stressfullifeevent,priordepression). Importantly,priordepression(ayes/noindicatorself-re- portedbytherespondent)wasassessedatwaveone,while thedepressivesymptomsoutcomewasassessedatwave two,whichallowedustocontrolforthetemporalordering betweencurrentandpriordepression.Table 1 provides additionalinformationaboutthesecontrolvariables. StatisticalAnalyses FollowingRaudenbushetal.(2003),Sampsonetal.(2005), andothers(ZimmermanandMessner 2010, 2011),we 7 Thesamefiveitemsthatmeasuresocialcohesionarealsoincluded inthecollectiveefficacymeasure.Webelievethisoverlapis conceptuallytolerableforthepurposesofourinquiry.First,wewere interestedintheeffectofcollectiveefficacyonbothdepressionand theIPV—depressionrelationship,andthus,neededtoincludethe measureofcollectiveefficacyasithasbeenexaminedinprior research(e.g.,Sampsonetal. 1997).Additionally,therehasbeen somerecentattentiontotheimportanceofsocialcohesionwithregard todepression(Mairetal. 2010)aswellasbyitselfasafacilitatorof positiveneighborhoodbehavior(e.g.,informalsocialcontrol,see Warner 2014).Wewereinterestedinitsuniqueeffects—apartfrom collectiveefficacy—andthereforechosetoincludeaseparate measureofsocialcohesioninouranalyses.Collinearitydidnot presentaproblem,aswemodeledcollectiveefficacyandsocial cohesionseparately. AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 347 123employamultilevellogisticregressionmodeltopredictthe oddsthatarespondentlivinginagivenneighborhoodwill reportexperiencingdepressivesymptoms.Thisallowsus toutilizeall34,766responsestothewavetwodepression symptomitemsprovidedbyall2959ofthefemalesliving in80neighborhoodsinoursample.Thus,ouranalytic techniqueincludesanyfemalewhorespondedtoatleast onedepressionsymptomitemaskedatwavetwo.This techniqueeffectivelyavoidsthelossofdataduetomissing itemresponses(Osgoodetal. 2002)andtakesitemdiffi- cultyintoaccount(i.e.,somedepressionsymptomsareless prevalentthanothers)(Sampsonetal. 2005). ThemultivariatemultilevelRaschmodelisathree-level modelinwhichdichotomousdepressionitemsarenested withinpersons,whoarenestedwithinneighborhoods (Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thelevel-oneoutcomeisthe log-oddsofrespondingaffirmativelytoitem i of m - 1 depressionitems,by j person,livingin k neighborhood. Table1 Descriptiveinformationfordependentandindependentvariables MeanSDMin– Max Dependentvariable DepressionSymptomsThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon13indicatorsofdepression(wave2).09.290to1 Individual-levelvariables SevereIPVSixitemmeasureindicatingwhetherthemalepartnerusedsevereaggressionagainstthe femalerespondentinthepastyear(hitwithafist,hitwithsomething,beatup,choked, threatenedwithaknifeorgun,orusedaknifeorgun)(wave1) .15.350to1 AgeAgeoffemalerespondent(wave1)32.727.9015.02to 80.93 HispanicFemalerespondentisHispanic(wave1).48.500to1 AfricanAmericanFemalerespondentisAfricanAmerican(wave1).31.460to1 OtherraceFemalerespondentisanotherrace/ethnicity(wave1).04.190to1 Caucasian(ref)FemalerespondentisCaucasian(wave1).16.370to1 MarriedFemalerespondentismarried(wave1).63.480to1 HouseholdsalaryMaximumhouseholdsalaryrangingfromlessthan$5000toover$50,000(wave1)4.011.951to7 UnemploymentFemalerespondentisunemployed(wave1).48.500to1 IsolationFive-itemscalereportedbythefemaleindicatinghersocialisolation(feelingalone,not havingfriendstotalkto,eachonascaleof1-3,indicatingverytruetonottrue)(wave1) (reliability = .613) -.001.00 -.92to 3.31 StressfullifeeventFemales’exposuretostressfullifeeventswherebyafriend,familymember,or acquaintancewashurt(e.g.,shotatorraped)orkilledbyaviolentact(waves1and2) .48.500to1 PriordepressionFemalerespondentreportedpriordepression(wave1).12.330to1 Neighborhood-levelvariables Concentrated disadvantage Principalcomponentsfactoranalysisusingthreeitems(reliability = .805)fromthe1990 Census:thepercentageofresidentsbelowpoverty,householdsreceivingpublic assistance,andresidentsunemployed .001.00 -1.51to 2.35 CollectiveefficacyThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon10indicatorsofsocialcohesionandinformal socialcontrolreportedbyadultresidentsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .847) -.01.22 -.46to .64 SocialcohesionThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon5indicatorsofsocialcohesionandtrustbetween neighborsreportedbyadultresidentsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .826) -.00.20 -.46to .64 SocialinteractionsThree-levelitemresponsemodelbasedon4indicatorsofinteractionsbetween neighborhoodsreportedbyadultsintheCommunitySurvey(reliability = .734) -.00.17 -.52to .63 AnyfriendsWhetheradultsintheCommunitySurveyreportedhavinganyfriendslivingintheir neighborhood.ResponseswereaggregatedtotheNC-level .83.08.62–.98 AnyfamilyWhetheradultsintheCommunitySurveyreportedhavinganyrelativesorin-lawslivingin theirneighborhood.ResponseswereaggregatedtotheNC-level .45.170–.86 Averagenumberof familyandfriends Numberofrelativesorin-lawsandfriendslivingintheneighborhoodasreportedbythe adultsintheCommunitySurvey(eachonascaleof0–4,indicating0–10ormore). ThesemeasureswereaveragedandthenaggregatedtotheNC-level 1.52.35.96–3.02 Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters 348 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123Thisvariablelocatesitemseveritiesonthelogitscale (Raudenbushetal. 2003).Thus,thelevel-onemodel adjuststhewithin-personpropensityfordepressivesymp- tomsbyitemseverity,missingdata,andmeasurement error.Thelevel-oneinterceptservesastheoutcomeforthe level-twoandlevel-threemodelsandisassumedtobe normallydistributedonalogitscale. Thelevel-twomodelexaminestheeffectsofperson- levelcorrelates(e.g.,age,maritalstatus,severeIPV)onthe level-oneintercept(susceptibilityfordepressivesymp- toms),whilealsocontrollingforitemseveritiesatlevel- one.Alloftheperson-leveleffectsweregrand-meancen- tered.Fortheneighborhooddirecteffectsmodels,the effectsofallcovariateswereassumedtobefixedacross neighborhoods;however,theanalysesexaminingthecross- levelinteractionsallowedtheeffectofexperiencingsevere IPVtovaryacrossneighborhoods(p\.05). Thelevel-threemodelallowsestimationofthesuscep- tibilityofdepressivesymptomsacrossneighborhoods.The level-threeintercepts-as-outcomesmodelexaminesthe directeffectsofneighborhooddisadvantageandsupportive mechanismsonthelevel-twointercept(i.e.,thesuscepti- bilityofexperiencingdepressionsymptoms,controllingfor person-levelcorrelatesatlevel-twoanditemseveritiesat level-one).Thelevel-threeslopes-and-intercepts-as-out- comesmodelallowsforexaminationofthecross-level interactionbetweenneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms andthelevel-tworelationshipbetweenexperiencingsevere IPVandsusceptibilityforexperiencingdepressionsymp- toms,whileadjustingfortheindividualandneighborhood maineffects.Multicollinearitywasnotaprobleminanyof themodels(tolerancevalueswereabove.40). Results Tables 2 and 3 presenttheresultsofourthreeresearch questions.Table 2 presentsthemaineffectofexperiencing severeIPVfromone’spartneratwaveoneondepressive symptomsapproximately3yearslater,controllingfor neighborhoodvariablesaswellasindividual-levelfactors, suchaspriordepressionandmaritalstatus.Theseanalyses donotincludethecross-levelinteractionbetweenneigh- borhoodsupportivemechanismsandtheIPV—depression relationship.Theresultsoftheindividual-levelmodels suggestthat,asexpected,experiencingsevereabusefrom one’spartnersignificantlyincreasesthelikelihoodthat femaleswillreportsubsequentdepressivesymptoms; importantly,thiseffectissignificantevenaftercontrolling fortheimpactofpriordepression.Additionally,struggling withpriordepression(comparedtonotreportingprior depressivesymptoms),experiencingastressfullifeevent (versusnotexperiencingsuchevents),andhavinghigher householdsalaries(asopposedtolowersalaries)increased thelikelihoodofexperiencingdepressivesymptomsacross allmodels.Beingmarried(asopposedtodatingor cohabitingwithapartner)protectedwomenagainstexpe- riencingdepressivesymptomsacrossallmodels. InthebottomhalfofTable 2,weprovidetheneigh- borhooddirecteffectsondepressivesymptoms,controlling forthemaineffectofneighborhooddisadvantageand respondent-compositionalfactorsinallanalyses.Resultsof theneighborhooddirecteffectsindicatethatmanyofthe neighborhoodsupportivemechanismsare,infact,protec- tiveagainstexperiencingdepressivesymptoms,evenwhen neighborhooddisadvantageandindividual-levelprior depressionaretakenintoaccount.Specifically,women whoresideinneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigherlevels ofcollectiveefficacy(Model1),socialcohesion(Model2), socialinteractionsbetweenresidents(Model3),and neighborhoodsinwhichmanypeoplehadfriendsand familymembers(Model6)werelesslikelytoreport experiencingdepressivesymptomsthanwomenresidingin neighborhoodscharacterizedbylesscollectiveefficacy, cohesion,networks,andfewerfriendsorfamilymembers onaverage.Neighborhoodconcentrateddisadvantagewas alsosignificantly(thoughmodestly)associatedwitha lowerlikelihoodofdepressivesymptomswhencollective efficacy(Model1)andsocialcohesion(Model2)werealso includedintheanalyses,thoughitwasnotdirectlyrelated todepressivesymptomsinanyoftheothermodels. InTable 3,weincludethecross-levelinteractions betweenneighborhoodsupportsandtheIPV—subsequent depressionrelationship.Thesearereportedintherows belowtheSevereIPVvariable.Theseinteractionsare assessedsimultaneouslywiththeindividualandneigh- borhoodmaineffects.Nosubstantivechangesintheindi- vidualorneighborhoodmaineffectswereobservedwhen thecross-levelinteractionwasincludedinTable 3.As mentionedabove,theimpactofexperiencingsevereIPV fromone’spartnerinfactvariedacrossneighborhoods,and wasallowedtovaryforthecross-levelinteractionmod- els(asindicatedbytheitalicizedcoefficients).Thepurpose ofexaminingthecross-levelinteractionswastodetermine ifneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,suchascollective efficacy,explainedthisvariation,perhapsbymitigatingthe impactofIPVondepressivesymptoms.Wefoundvery littleevidencethatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms alleviatedtheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepression,and theonlytwosignificantmoderatingeffectswefoundwere somewhatcounterintuitive.Model5inTable 3 indicates thattheimpactofexperiencingsevereIPVonlater depressivesymptomswassignificantlystrongerforwomen whoresidedinneighborhoodscharacterizedbygreater proportionsoffamilymembersintheneighborhood(as reportedbyrespondentsoftheCommunitySurvey,notthe AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 349 123respondentsofIPV),whileModel6demonstratesthatthe influenceofIPVondepressivesymptomswasalsostronger forwomenlivinginneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigher averagenumbersoffamilyandfriendslivinginthesame neighborhood(thoughthiseffectwasmodest,reaching significanceatthe p B .10level). Figure 1 depictsthecross-levelinteractiondisplayedin Model5ofTable 3,whileFig. 2 depictsthecross-level interactioninModel6.Asshown,therelationshipbetween IPVandthelikelihoodofexperiencingdepressivesymp- tomswassteepandpositiveinneighborhoodscharacter- izedbyhigh(onestandarddeviationabovethemean) familyassociates(Fig. 1)andfriendsandfamilymembers (Fig. 2)intheneighborhood.TheimpactofIPVon depressivesymptomsweakened(asindicatedbytheslope tiltinginthenegativedirection)inareascharacterizedby low(onestandarddeviationbelowthemean)family members(Fig. 1)andfamilyandfriends(Fig. 2).Thus, althoughweexpectedthatneighborhoodsupportive mechanisms,suchastieswithothers,wouldpotentially shieldvictimsfromexperiencingdepressivesymptoms,we foundtheoppositeeffect–insomeneighborhoods,par- ticularlythosecharacterizedbyhighlevelsoffamily members,theimpactofbeingavictimofpartnerviolence onlaterdepressivesymptomswasactuallyworse. Discussion Manyscholarsagreethatneighborhoodcontextisimpor- tantwhenitcomestobothintimatepartnerviolenceand depression(orothermentalhealthproblems)(e.g.,Benson etal. 2003;Goodmanetal. 2009;KimandRoss 2009),and anumberofstudieshaveexaminedthenegativeordetri- mentalimpactsofneighborhoodswithregardtothese outcomes(e.g.,Mairetal. 2008;Miles-Doan 1998). Table2 Directeffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceandneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsondepressionsymptoms Model1Model2Model3Model4Model5Model6 b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE) Intercept -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.87**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) Individual-leveleffects Age.00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01) Hispanic.25(.15).24(.16).26(.15).30*(.15).32*(.15).30*(.15) AfricanAmerican.06(.21).08(.21).05(.21).05(.21).07(.21).03(.22) Otherrace.07(.28).08(.29).07(.28).08(.28).07(.27).06(.28) Married -.31* (.13) -.30*(.13) -.31*(.13) -.32*(.13) -.32*(.13) -.32*(.13) Householdsalary.07* (.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03) Unemployment.09(.14).09(.14).10(.14).09(.14).08(.14).09(.14) Isolation.06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).07(.07).06(.07) Stressfullifeevent.38** (.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11).38**(.11) Priordepression1.07** (.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18)1.08**(.18) SevereIPV.50** (.15).50**(.16).51**(.16).50**(.15).50**(.15).50**(.15) Neighborhooddirecteffects Concentrateddisadvantage -.15 (.08) -.16*(.07) -.07(.08) -.09(.08) -.07(.08) -.07(.08) Collectiveefficacy -.59* (.24)–––––––––– Socialcohesion–– -.93**(.31)–––––––– Socialinteraction–––– -.61*(.28)–––––– Anyfriends–––––– -.75(.60)–––– Anyfamily–––––––– -.25(.31)–– Averagenumberoffamilyand friends –––––––––– -.23*(.11) Variancecomponents Individuallevelintercept7.7947.8037.7997.7947.7907.793 Neighborhoodintercept.205.173.195.207.215.206 Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters ** p B .01;* p B .05;  p B .10 350 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123However,lessattentionhasbeenpaidtothepotential protectiveorhealth-promotingeffectsthatsometypesof neighborhoodsprovidefortheirresidents(forexceptions, seeforinstance,AhernandGalea 2011;Kim 2010;Kim andRoss 2009;Mairetal. 2008, 2010;RossandJang 2000;Staffordetal. 2011).Additionally,theinterplayof partnerviolence,neighborhoodsupportivemechanisms, anddepressivesymptoms,toourknowledge,hasnotbeen assessed,limitingourunderstandingforhowtheIPV— depressionrelationshipoperateswhenneighborhoodcon- textisconsidered.Ourstudysoughttoadvancethisliter- aturebyexploringwhetherneighborhoodsupportive mechanismsalleviatedthedetrimentalimpactofIPVon subsequentdepressivesymptoms.Ourresultssuggestthat theimpactofviolentrelationshipsonlatermentalhealth outcomesmaydependinpartonneighborhoodcharacter- istics,particularlysocialties,butcontrarytoourexpecta- tions,thepresenceofhighlevelsofneighborhoodties Table3 Effectsofintimatepartnerviolenceandneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsondepressionsymptoms,andcross-levelinteractions betweenneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsandtheIPV—depressionrelationship Model1Model2Model3Model4Model5Model6 b(SE)b(SE)B(SE)b(SE)b(SE)b(SE) Intercept -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.86**(.06) -1.85**(.06) Individual-leveleffectsandcross-levelinteractions Age.00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01).00(.01) Hispanic.22(.15).21(.15).23(.14).27(.15).30*(.14).27(.14) AfricanAmerican.06(.20).08(.20).06(.21).05(.21).08(.21).05(.21) Otherrace.01(.28).02(.28) -.01(.27).02(.28) -.02(.26) -.03(.27) Married -.33* (.13) -.32*(.13) -.33*(.13) -.34*(.13) -.34**(.13) -.34**(.13) Householdsalary.07* (.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03).07*(.03) Unemployment.08(.14).09(.14).10(.14).08(.14).08(.14).09(.14) Isolation.06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07).06(.07) Stressfullifeevent.37** (.11).37**(.11).37**(.11).38**(.11).37**(.11).37**(.11) Priordepression1.09** (.17)1.09**(.17)1.09**(.17)1.10**(.17)1.10**(.17)1.09**(.17) SevereIPV .50** (.16) .50** (.17) .51** (.16) .48** (.16) .44** (.15) .48** (.16) 9 Collectiveefficacy.03(.81)–––––––––– 9 Socialcohesion––.24(1.04)–––––––– 9 Socialinteraction––––1.02(.79)–––––– 9 Anyfriends–––––– -1.14(1.64)–––– 9 Anyfamily––––––––1.91*(.91)–– 9 Averagenumberoffamily andfriends ––––––––––.63 (.34) Neighborhoodmaineffects Concentrateddisadvantage -.16 (.08) -.16*(.07) -.07(.08) -.10(.08) -.07(.08) -.07(.08) Collectiveefficacy -.60*(.24)–––––––––– Socialcohesion–– -.92**(.32)–––––––– Socialinteraction–––– -.65*(.28)–––––– Anyfriends–––––– -.74(.62)–––– Anyfamily–––––––– -.25(.31)–– Averagenumberoffamilyand friends –––––––––– -.22*(.11) Variancecomponents Individual-levelintercept7.6137.6227.6227.6167.6187.620 Neighborhoodintercept.207.175.202.211.220.214 SevereIPVintercept1.8911.8721.8181.8281.6611.729 Basedon34,766responsesacross2959respondentswithin80neighborhoodclusters Thesignificanceofitalicvalueindicatesrandomlyvaryingcoefficient ** p B .01;* p B .05;  p B .10; AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 351 123appearedtostrengthenratherthanattenuatetherelation- shipbetweenIPVanddepressivesymptoms.Wedidfind, however,thatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms,par- ticularlythoserelatedtocollectiveefficacy,cohesion,and socialinteractionswithneighbors,alleviatedthelikelihood ofexperiencingsymptomsofdepressionevenaftercon- trollingfortheeffectsofpriordepression.Wediscussour findingsbelow. Ourfirstgoalwastoexaminethelong-termimpactof IPVondepressivesymptomsusinglongitudinaldata,as fewstudieshaveassessedtheprospectiveimpactofviolent relationshipsonsuchanoutcome(Campbell 2002;Fletcher 2010;Johnsonetal. 2014).Wefoundthatexperiencing severeIPVsignificantlyincreasedthelikelihoodthat womenreporteddepressivesymptomsyearslater.The relationshipbetweenIPVanddepressionwasrobust, maintainingasignificanteffectdespitehighlyspecified modelswhichincludedseveralotherimportantcovariates, suchaspriordepressionand,perhapsevenmoreimpor- tantly,whilesimultaneouslyinvestigatingcontextual influences(Goodmanetal. 2009).Giventhestrengthofour multilevellongitudinalmodels,ourresultsdemonstratethat IPVisasignificantriskfactorforsubsequentmentalhealth problemsthatisindependentfromtheeffectsofother predictorsofdepression,suchaspriordepression,stressful lifeevents,andisolation.ThatIPVmaintainedastrong longitudinaleffectondepressioninthesemodelssuggests thatitremainsacentralpublichealthconcern(seealso Bonomietal. 2006;CaetanoandCunradi 2003;DeMaris andKaukinen 2005;Golding 1999). -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 High Proporon of Family Members in Neighborhood Average Proporon of Family Members in Neighborhood Low Proporon of Family Members in Neighborhood Likelihood of Depression Symptoms Severe Inmate Partner Violence 0 1 Fig.1 Therelationship betweensevereintimatepartner violenceanddepression symptomsbyneighborhood proportionoffamilyties. Controlsforneighborhood concentrateddisadvantageand allrespondent-levelcovariates -3 -2 -1 0 High Levels of Family and Friends in Neighborhood Average Levels of Family and Friends in Neighborhood Low Levels of Family and Friends in Neighborhood Likelihood of Depression Symptoms Severe Inmate Partner Violence 0 1 Fig.2 Therelationship betweensevereintimatepartner violenceanddepression symptomsbyaveragenumber offamilyandfriendsin neighborhood.Controlsfor concentrateddisadvantageand allrespondent-levelcovariates 352 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123Ournextgoalsweretoexamineseveralneighborhood- levelprotectivefactors—particularlythosemeasuring socialconnectionsandcohesivenessamongneighbors— whichhavebeensuggestedasmechanismsthatmight alleviatementalhealthsymptomsand/ortheeffectsof abuse(AhernandGalea 2011;Burkeetal. 2006;Mairetal. 2008;O’Campoetal. 2005;Staffordetal. 2011).We soughttoexamineboththedirectandmoderatinginflu- encesoftheseneighborhoodcharacteristicswhilecontrol- lingforimportantindividual-levelcovariates.Consistent withpriorresearchreportingadirectinfluenceofneigh- borhoodsondepression(e.g.,Kim 2010;Mairetal. 2008), wefoundthatneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsdo appeartoprotectindividualsfromexperiencingsymptoms ofdepression.Wesuspectthatneighborhoodfactorssuch ascollectiveefficacy,socialcohesionbetweenneighbors, socialinteractionsamongresidents,andhavingmore familyandfriendswhoresideinone’sneighborhoodare protectiveagainstnegativementalhealthsymptoms becausetheyprovidesupport,increasecommunication, andlowerfeelingsofisolation(e.g.,Browning 2002; Sampson 2013;Vegaetal. 2011).Assourcesofsupport, thesetiesmightalsoreducetheeffectsofstress,increase feelingsofcontrol,fosteraltruisticbehaviors,orreduce feelingsoffearormistrust,allofwhichinturnmight alleviatefeelingsofdepression(AhernandGalea 2011; KimandRoss 2009;RossandJang 2000;Sampson 2013; Staffordetal. 2011). However,wealsofoundthatsomeneighborhoodsup- portivemechanismsarenotnecessarilyprotectiveagainst depression,particularlywhenpartnerviolenceiscon- cerned.Livinginneighborhoodswheregreaterproportions offamilymembers(relativesorin-laws)residedactually exacerbatedthenegativeimpactofIPVondepressive symptomsamongfemales.Likewise,wefoundthatwomen wholivedinneighborhoodscharacterizedbyhigherpro- portionsoffamilyandfriendtiesweremoreat-riskfor depressivesymptomsfollowingIPVthanwomenwhose neighborhoodswerecharacterizedbyfewersuch‘‘sup- ports.’’Whilethe‘‘family’’elementmaybelargelydriving thislastfinding,westillconsiderbotheffectsnoteworthy andunexpected.Althoughneighborhoodsocialties—par- ticularlybetweenfamilyand/orfriends—islargelydeemed aprotectivemechanismformanyoutcomes(e.g.,Bellair 1997;WellmanandWortley 1990),includingbothpartner violence(e.g.,Agoffetal. 2007;Wright 2012)and depression(e.g.,KimandRoss 2009),wefoundthe oppositetobetrue,atleastregardingtheimpactofIPVon depressivesymptoms.Afewpossibilitiesmayaccountfor thesecontradictoryfindings.RecallthattheCommunity Surveysampledrespondentsfromneighborhoodswho werenotnecessarilyparticipantsoftheLCS—thus, ‘‘family’’tiesintheneighborhoodlikelydonotreflectthe victim’sownfamilymembers—theysimplyreflectthat suchtiesexistwithintheneighborhood.Inthiscase, womenwhoarebeingvictimizedmaynothavetheirown familymemberslivingcloseby.Indeed,itispossiblethat theIPVvictimsintheseneighborhoodsactuallymayhave feltdoublyisolatediftheyhadnolocalfamilyorfriends whileeveryonearoundthemwasstronglytiedintoalocal familial(orfriendship)network.Wesoughttotakethis possibilityintoaccount,however,byincludingthe females’levelofisolationasanindividual-levelcontrol. Thatitwasnotsignificantinourmodelssuggestsanother explanationiswarranted.Forinstance,wedonotknowif thevictimizedfemaleactuallyutilizedthesetiesforhelp. Whilesome(Browning 2002;VanWyketal. 2003;Wright andBenson 2011)havesuggestedthatmoresocialtiesmay makeitmorelikelythattheviolencewillbecomepublic knowledge,itmaybethatthesetiesweresimplynot activatedorsoughtoutforhelp(WrightandBenson 2011). SinceIPVisoftentimesconsideredveryprivateinnature (Strausetal. 2006),thisiscertainlyapossibility.Itisalso possiblethatbeingsurroundedbymorefamilyties(evenif notone’sownfamilymembers)inone’sneighborhoodputs pressureonwomentostayintherelationship,especiallyif familiesintheareadonotcondonedivorceorseparation, evenfromaviolentpartner(Agoffetal. 2007).Alterna- tively,itispossiblethatinthecontextofpartnerviolence, somesocialsupportsarenothelpful,andmayinfactbe harmfulbysupportingorcondoningtheuseofviolence withinrelationships.Weareunabletoexaminewhetherthe neighborhoodtiesmeasuredherearesupportiveofIPV, andthereforecannottestthispotentialexplanation.Ifthe socialtiesintheneighborhoodswerebetweenresidents whoweresupportiveofviolenceinrelationships,thiscould explaintheexacerbatingeffectfoundbetweenneighbor- hoodsocialtiesandtheimpactofIPVonlaterdepression. Wecannotbesurewhichexplanation,ifany,iscorrect giventheavailabledata,butweencouragefutureresearch toattempttodisentanglethecomplexrelationshipthat appearstoexistbetweenneighborhood-levelsocialties (especiallywithfamilyorrelatives)andpartnerviolence. Asidefromtheeffectoffamilyandfriends,wefoundno otherevidencethatneighborhoodsupportivemechanisms (collectiveefficacy,socialcohesion,socialinteraction) moderatedtheimpactofIPVonsubsequentdepression. Perhapstheneighborhoodsupportivemechanismsexam- inedherearetoogeneraltoalleviatetheeffectsofIPVon mentalhealth—itispossiblethatotherneighborhood constructsgearedmorespecificallytoviolenceandmental healthsupport,intervention,and/orprevention(e.g., counselingservices,shelters,etc.)wouldbemorerelevant. Futureresearchshouldconsiderthepotentialforsuch neighborhoodsupportivemechanismstoeasetheeffectsof specificviolencestressorssuchasIPVandabuseon AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 353 123negativementalhealthoutcomessothataggregatepre- ventionandinterventionscanbeimplemented(seealso Cunradi 2010). Limitations Unfortunately,ourstudyisnotwithoutlimitations.We reliedsolelyondatafromfemalecaregiversanddidnot includemalesaspotentialvictimsofIPV.Therefore,wedo notknowifourfindingswouldgeneralizetofemales’ violenceperpetratedagainstmales.Similarly,ourstudy doesnotexaminedatingviolencebyadolescents.Whilewe examinedtheimpactoftheprevalenceofsevereIPVon females’depression,wedidnotexaminetheimpactofthe frequencyorchronicityofsuchviolence.Itispossiblethat morefrequentormoresevereviolence,whileperhapsmore detrimentaltodepression(Campbell 2002),couldbe impactedbyneighborhoodfactorsdifferently.Finally,our datawerecollectedfromwomeninonecity(Chicago) duringthemid-1990sandintotheearly2000s;although thePHDCNisahighlyrespecteddataset,wecannotensure thatourresultswouldgeneralizetootherpopulationsor timeperiods. Ourstudyaddstoburgeoningevidencethatneighbor- hoodcontextimpactsvariousinterpersonaloutcomes,in bothdirectandmoderatingways.Itappearsthatneigh- borhoodsupportivemechanismsmaydirectlyprotect femalesfromnegativementalhealthproblemssuchas experiencingdepressivesymptoms.Theseneighborhood supportivemechanisms,thoughlessoftenconsidered,are importanttoexamineforpreventionandinterventionpur- poses,andsuggestthatjustasneighborhoodscanbe detrimental,theycanalsobepositiveandprotective. Neighborhoodcollectiveefficacy,cohesion,andinteraction betweenneighborsmightincreasefeelingsofsupport, belongingness,investment,communication,andother positiveattributes,whichmayinturnreducedepressive symptoms.Webelieveitisimportantforresearchersto continuetoexaminethepositiveandpotentiallymitigating influencesofneighborhoods,aswellastheirdetrimental effects,inordertobetterunderstandforwhomandunder whichcircumstancesviolentrelationshipsandmental healthareinfluencedbycontextualfactors. References Agoff,C.,Herrera,C.,&Castro,R.(2007).Theweaknessoffamily tiesandtheirperpetuatingeffectsongenderviolence:A qualitativestudyinMexico. ViolenceAgainstWomen,13(11), 1206–1220. Ahern,J.,&Galea,S.(2011).Collectiveefficacyandmajor depressioninurbanneighborhoods. AmericanJournalof Epidemiology,173(12),1453–1462. Aneshensel,C.S.,&Sucoff,C.A.(1996).Theneighborhoodcontext ofadolescentmentalhealth. JournalofHealthandSocial Behavior,37(4),293–310. Bellair,P.E.(1997).Socialinteractionandcommunitycrime: Examiningtheimportanceofneighbornetworks. Criminology, 35(4),677–704. Benson,M.L.,Fox,G.L.,DeMaris,A.,&VanWyk,J.(2003). Neighborhooddisadvantage,individualeconomicdistressand violenceagainstwomeninintimaterelationships. Journalof QuantitativeCriminology,19(3),207–235. Bonomi,A.E.,Thompson,R.S.,Anderson,M.,Reid,R.J.,Carrell, D.,Dimer,J.A.,&Rivara,F.P.(2006).Intimatepartner violenceandwomen’sphysical,mental,andsocialfunctioning. AmericanJournalofPreventiveMedicine,30(6),458–466. Browning,C.R.(2002).Thespanofcollectiveefficacy:Extending socialdisorganizationtheorytopartnerviolence. Journalof MarriageandFamily,64(4),833–850. Browning,C.R.,Feinberg,S.L.,&Dietz,R.D.(2004).Theparadox ofsocialorganization:Networks,collectiveefficacy,andviolent crimeinurbanneighborhoods. SocialForces,83(2),503–534. Burke,J.,O’Campo,P.,&Peak,G.L.(2006).Neighborhood influencesandintimatepartnerviolence:Doesgeographic settingmatter? JournalofUrbanHealth,83(2),182–194. Burke,J.,O’Campo,P.,Salmon,C.,&Walker,R.(2009).Pathways connectingneighborhoodinfluencesandmentalwell-being: Socioeconomicpositionandgenderdifferences. SocialScience andMedicine,68,1294–1304. Caetano,R.,&Cunradi,C.(2003).Intimatepartnerviolenceand depressionamongWhites,Blacks,andHispanics. Annalsof Epidemiology,13,661–665. Caldwell,J.E.,Swan,S.C.,&Woodbrown,V.D.(2012).Gender differencesinintimatepartnerviolenceoutcomes. Psychologyof Violence,2(1),42–57. Campbell,J.C.(2002).Healthconsequencesofintimatepartner violence. Lancet,359(9341),1331–1336. Campbell,J.C.,Kub,J.E.,&Rose,L.(1996).Depressioninbattered women. JournaloftheAmericanMedicalWomen’sAssociation, 51(3),106–110. Cerda,M.,Sanchez,B.N.,Galea,S.,Tracy,M.,&Buka,S.L. (2008).Estimatingco-occuringbehavioraltrajectorieswithina neighborhoodcontext:Acasestudyofmultivariatetransition modelsforclustereddata. AmericanJournalofEpidemiology, 168(10),1190–1203. Coker,A.L.,Davis,K.E.,Arias,I.,Desai,R.A.,Sanderson,M., Brandt,H.M.,&Smith,P.H.(2002a).Physicalandmental healtheffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceformenandwomen. AmericanJournalofPreventiveMedicine,23(4),260–268. Coker,A.L.,Smith,P.H.,Thompson,M.P.,McKeown,R.E., Bethea,L.,&Davis,K.E.(2002b).Socialsupportprotects againstthenegativeeffectsofpartnerviolenceonmentalhealth. JournalofWomen’sHealth&Gender-BasedMedicine,11(5), 465–476. Cunradi,C.(2010).Neighborhoods,alcoholoutletsandintimate partnerviolence:Addressingresearchgapsinexplanatory mechanisms. InternationalJournalofEnvironmentalResearch andPublicHealth,7,799–813. DeMaris,A.,&Kaukinen,C.(2005).Violentvictimizationand women’smentalandphysicalhealth:Evidencefromanational sample. JournalofResearchinCrimeandDelinquency,42(4), 384–411. Earls,F.J.,Brooks-Gunn,J.,Raudenbush,S.W.,&Sampson,R.J. (2002). ProjectonhumandevelopmentinChicago 354 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123neighborhoods(PHDCN).AnnArbor,MI:Inter-university CorsortiumforPoliticalandSocialResearch. Echeverria,S.,DiezRoux,A.V.,Shea,S.,Borrell,L.N.,&Jackson, S.(2008).Associationsofneighborhoodproblemsandneigh- borhoodsocialcohesionwithmentalhealthandhealthbehav- iors:Themulti-ethnicstudyofatherosclerosis. Health&Place, 14(4),853–865. Edwards,K.M.,Mattingly,M.J.,Dixon,K.J.,&Banyard,V.L. (2014).Communitymatters:Intimatepartnerviolenceamong ruralyoungadults. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology, 53,198–207. Emery,C.R.,Jolley,J.M.,&Wu,S.(2011).Desistancefrom intimatepartnerviolence:Theroleoflegalcynicism,collective efficacy,andsocialdisorganizationinChicagoneighborhoods. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,48,373–383. Fletcher,J.(2010).Theeffectsofintimatepartnerviolenceonhealth inyoungadulthoodintheUnitedStates. SocialScienceand Medicine,70,130–135. Geis,K.J.,&Ross,C.E.(1998).Anewlookaturbanalienation:The effectofneighborhooddisorderonperceivedpowerlessness. SocialPsychologyQuarterly,61(3),232–246. Golding,J.M.(1999).Intimatepartnerviolenceasariskfactorfor mentaldisorders:Ameta-analysis. JournalofFamilyViolence, 14(2),99–132. Goodman,L.A.,Smyth,K.F.,Borges,A.M.,&Singer,R.(2009). Whencrisescollide:Howintimatepartnerviolenceandpoverty intersecttoshapewomen’smentalhealthandcoping? Trauma, Violence,&Abuse,10(4),306–329. Hadeed,L.F.,&El-Bassel,N.(2006).SocialsupportamongAfro- Trinidadianwomenexperiencingintimatepartnerviolence. ViolenceAgainstWomen,12(8),740–760. Hill,T.D.,Ross,C.E.,&Angel,R.J.(2005).Neighborhood disorder,psychophysiologicaldistress,andhealth. Journalof HealthandSocialBehavior,46(2),170–186. Johnson,W.L.,Giordano,P.C.,Longmore,M.A.,&Manning,W. D.(2014).Intimatepartnerviolenceanddepressivesymptoms duringadolescenceandyoungadulthood. JournalofHealthand SocialBehavior,55(1),39–55. Kessler,R.C.,Andrews,G.,Mroczerk,D.,Ustun,B.,&Wittchen, H.-U.(1998).TheWorldHealthOrganizationcompositeinter- nationaldiagnosticinterviewshort-form(CIDI-SF). Interna- tionalJournalofMethodsinPsychiatricResearch,7(4), 171–185. Kim,D.(2008).Bluesfromtheneighborhood?Neighborhood characteristicsanddepression. EpidemiologicReviews,30, 101–117. Kim,J.(2010).Neighborhooddisadvantageandmentalhealth:The roleofneighborhooddisorderandsocialrelationships. Social ScienceResearch,39(2),260–271. Kim,J.,&Ross,C.E.(2009).Neighborhood-specificandgeneral socialsupport:Whichbufferstheeffectofneighborhood disorderondepression? JournalofCommunityPsychology, 37(6),725–736. Kirk,D.S.,&Matsuda,M.(2011).Legalcynicism,collective efficacy,andtheecologyofarrest. Criminology,49(2),443–472. Kirst,M.,Lazgare,L.P.,Zhang,Y.J.,&O’Campo,P.(2015).The effectsofsocialcapitalandneighborhoodcharacteristicson intimatepartnerviolence:Aconsiderationofsocialresources andrisks. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,55, 314–325. Kornhauser,R.R.(1978). Socialsourcesofdelinquency:An appraisalofanalyticmodels.Chicago,IL:UniversityofChicago Press. Latkin,C.A.,&Curry,A.D.(2003).Stressfulneighborhoodsand depression:Aprospectivestudyoftheimpactofneighborhood disorder. JournalofHealthandSocialBehavior,44(1),34–44. Mair,C.,DiezRoux,A.V.,&Galea,S.(2008).Areneighbourhood characteristicsassociatedwithdepressivesymptoms?Areview ofevidence. JournalofEpidemiologyandCommunityHealth, 62(11),940–946. Mair,C.,DiezRoux,A.V.,&Morenoff,J.D.(2010).Neighborhood stressorsandsocialsupportaspredictorsofdepressivesymp- tomsintheChicagoCommunityAdultHealthStudy. Health& Place,16(5),811–819. Miles-Doan,R.(1998).Violencebetweenspousesandintimates:Does neighborhoodcontextmatter? SocialForces,77(2),623–645. Moe,A.M.(2007).Silencedvoicesandstructuredsurvival:Battered women’shelpseeking. ViolenceAgainstWomen,13(7),676–699. Molnar,B.E.,Cerda,M.,Roberts,A.L.,&Buka,S.L.(2008). Effectsofneighborhoodresourcesonaggressiveanddelinquent behaviorsamongurbanyouths. AmericanJournalofPublic Health,98(6),1086–1093. Molnar,B.E.,Miller,M.J.,Azrael,D.,&Buka,S.L.(2004). Neighborhoodpredictorsofconcealedfirearmcarryingamong childrenandadolescents:Resultsfromtheprojectonhuman developmentinChicagoneighborhoods. ArchivesofPediatrics andAdolescentMedicine,158(7),657–664. Morenoff,J.D.,Sampson,R.J.,&Raudenbush,S.W.(2001). Neighborhoodinequality,collectiveefficacy,andthespatial dynamicsofurbanviolence. Criminology,39(3),517–559. O’Campo,P.,Burke,J.,Peak,G.L.,McDonnell,K.A.,&Gielen,A. C.(2005).Uncoveringneighbourhoodinfluenceonintimate partnerviolenceusingconceptmapping. JournalofEpidemiol- ogyandCommunityHealth,59(7),603–608. Osgood,D.W.,McMorris,B.J.,&Potenza,M.T.(2002).Analyzing multiple-itemmeasuresofcrimeanddevianceI:Itemresponse theoryscaling. JournalofQuantitativeCriminology,18, 267–296. Pinchevsky,G.M.,&Wright,E.M.(2012).Theimpactof neighborhoodsonintimatepartnerviolenceandvictimization. Trauma,Violence,&Abuse,12(2),112–132. Raudenbush,S.W.,Johnson,C.,&Sampson,R.J.(2003).A multivariate,multilevelRaschmodelwithapplicationtoself- reportedcriminalbehavior. SociologicalMethodology,33(1), 169–211. Rios,R.,Aiken,L.S.,&Zautra,A.J.(2012).Neighborhoodcontexts andthemediatingroleofneighborhoodsocialcohesionon healthandpsychologicaldistressamongHispanicandnon- Hispanicresidents. AnnalsofBehavioralMedicine,43(1), 50–61.doi:10.1007/s12160-011-9306-9. Ross,C.E.(2000).Neighborhooddisadvantageandadultdepression. JournalofHealthandSocialBehavior,41(2),177–187. Ross,C.E.,&Jang,S.J.(2000).Neighborhooddisorder,fear,and mistrust:Thebufferingroleofsocialtieswithneighbors. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,28(4),401–420. Ross,C.E.,&Mirowsky,J.(2009).Neighborhooddisorder, subjectivealienation,anddistress. JournalofHealthandSocial Behavior,50(1),49–64.doi:10.1177/002214650905000104. Ross,C.E.,Mirowsky,J.,&Pribesh,S.(2001).Powerlessandthe amplificationofthreat:Neighborhooddisadvantage,disorder, andmistrust. AmericanSociologicalReview,66(4),568–591. Ross,C.E.,Reynolds,J.R.,&Geis,K.J.(2000).Thecontingent meaningofneighborhoodstabilityforresidents’psychological well-being. AmericanSociologicalReview,65(4),581–597. Sampson,R.J.(2003).Theneighborhoodcontextofwellbeing. PerspectivesinBiologyandMedicine,46(3),S53–S73. Sampson,R.J.(2013). GreatAmericancity:Chicagoandthe enduringneighborhoodeffect.Chicago,IL:Universityof ChicagoPress. Sampson,R.J.,Morenoff,J.D.,&Raudenbush,S.W.(2005).Social anatomyofracialandethnicdisparatiesinviolence. American JournalofPublicHealth,95(2),224–232. AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 355 123Sampson,R.J.,Raudenbush,S.W.,&Earls,F.J.(1997). Neighborhoodsandviolentcrime:Amultilevelstudyof collectiveefficacy. Science,277(5328),918–924. Shaw,C.R.,&McKay,H.D.(1942). Juveniledelinquencyandurban areas:Astudyofratesofdelinquentsinrelationtodifferential characteristicsoflocalcommunitiesinAmericancities.Chicago, IL:UniversityofChicagoPress. Stafford,M.,McMunn,A.,&DeVogli,R.(2011).Neighbourhood socialenvironmentanddepressivesymptomsinmid-lifeand beyond. Ageing&Society,31(6),893–910. Stith,S.M.,Smith,D.B.,Penn,C.E.,Ward,D.B.,&Tritt,D. (2004).Intimatepartnerphysicalabuseperpetrationandvictim- izationriskfactors:Ameta-analyticreview. Aggressionand ViolentBehavior,10(1),65–98. Straus,M.A.(1979).Measuringintrafamilyconflictandviolence: Theconflicttactics(CT)scales. JournalofMarriageand Family,41(1),75–88. Straus,M.A.,Gelles,R.J.,&Steinmetz,S.K.(2006). Behindclosed doors:ViolenceintheAmericanfamily.NewBrunswick,NJ: TransactionPublishers. Taylor,S.E.,&Repetti,R.L.(1997).Healthpsychology:Whatisan unhealthyenvironmentandhowdoesitgetundertheskin? AnnualReviewofPsychology,48,411–447. VanWyk,J.A.,Benson,M.L.,Fox,G.L.,&DeMaris,A.(2003). Detanglingindividual-,partner-,andcommunity-levelcorrelates ofpartnerviolence. Crime&Delinquency,49(3),412–438. Vega,W.A.,Ang,A.,Rodriguez,M.A.,&Finch,B.K.(2011). NeighborhoodprotectiveeffectsondepressioninLatinos. AmericanJournalofCommunityPsychology,47(1–2),114–126. Warner,B.D.(2014).Neighborhoodfactorsrelatedtothelikelihood ofsuccessfulinformalsocialcontrolefforts. JournalofCriminal Justice,42,421–430. Wellman,B.,&Wortley,S.(1990).Differentstrokesfromdifferent folks:Communitytiesandsocialsupport. AmericanJournalof Sociology,96(3),558–588. Wilson,J.Q.,&Kelling,G.L.(1982).Thepoliceandneighborhood safety:Brokenwindows. AtlanticMonthly,127,29–38. Wright,E.M.(2012).Therelationshipbetweensocialsupportand intimatepartnerviolenceinneighborhoodcontext. Crime& Delinquency.doi:10.1177/0011128712466890. Wright,E.M.,&Benson,M.L.(2010).Immigrationandintimate partnerviolence:Exploringtheimmigrantparadox. Social Problems,57(3),480–503. Wright,E.M.,&Benson,M.L.(2011).Clarifyingtheeffectsof neighborhooddisadvantageandcollectiveefficacyonviolence ‘‘behindcloseddoors’’. JusticeQuarterly,28(5),775–798. Xue,Y.,Leventhal,T.,Brooks-Gunn,J.,&Earls,F.J.(2005). Neighborhoodresidenceandmentalhealthproblemsof5-to 11-yearolds. ArchivesofGeneralPsychiatry,62(5),554–563. Zimmerman,G.M.,&Messner,S.F.(2010).Neighborhoodcontext andthegendergapinadolescentviolentcrime. American SociologicalReview,75(6),958–980. Zimmerman,G.M.,&Messner,S.F.(2011).Neighborhoodcontext andnonlinearpeereffectsonadolescentviolentcrime. Crimi- nology,49(3),873–903. 356 AmJCommunityPsychol(2015)56:342–356 123Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.